Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-06T07:46:55.042Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2018

Alberto Rosa
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Jaan Valsiner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Ash, M. G. (1995). Gestalt Psychology in German Culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the Quest for Objectivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 11771187. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1319.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1925). Feeling, imaging and thinking. British Journal of Psychology, 16, 1628.Google Scholar
Bertalanffy, L. von (1968/1976). General System Theory: Foundations, Development, Applications (rev. edn). New York: George Braziller.Google Scholar
Billard, A. & Dautenhahn, K. (1998). Grounding communication in autonomous robots: An experimental study. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 24(1–2), 7179.Google Scholar
Billard, A. & Hayes, G. (1997). Robot's first steps, robot's first words. In Sorace, P. & Heycock, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ‘97 Conference on Language Acquisition, Edinburgh. Edinburgh: Human Communication Research Centre.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (Eds.). (1996). Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 2562.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (1st edn). London: John Murray. Retrieved from http://darwin-online.org.uk/content/frameset?pageseq=1&itemID=F1142&viewtype=text.Google Scholar
Diriwächter, R. (2004). Völkerpsychologie: The synthesis that never was. Culture & Psychology, 10(1), 179203.Google Scholar
Diriwächter, R. & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2008). Striving for the Whole: Creating Theoretical Syntheses. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Innis, R. (2016). Affective semiosis: Philosophical links to a cultural psychology. In Valsiner, J., Marsico, G., Chaudhary, G., Sato, T. & Dazzani, V. (Eds.), Psychology as a Science of Human Being: The Yokohama Manifesto. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1992). Crossroads between Culture and Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. (1979). Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Psychology. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Kull, K. (2011). Yuri Lotman in English: Bibliography. Sign Systems Studies, 39(2/4), 343356.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (1996). La semiosfera I: Semiótica de la cultura y del texto (trans. from Russian by Desiderio Navarro). Madrid: Cátedra.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (1998). La semiosfera II: Semiótica de la cultura y del texto de la conducta y del espacio (trans. from Russian by Desiderio Navarro). Madrid: Cátedra.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (2000). La semiosfera III. Semiótica de las artes y de la cultura (trans. from Russian by Desiderio Navarro). Madrid: Cátedra.Google Scholar
Lotman, J. (2005). On the semiosphere. Sign Systems Studies, 33(1), 206229 (trans. by Wilma Clark).Google Scholar
Mammen, J. (2016). Using a topological model in psychology: Developing sense and choice categories. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50(2), 196233.Google Scholar
Martin, J. & Gillespie, A. (2010). A neo-Meadian approach to human agency: Relating the social and the psychological in the ontogenesis of perspective-coordinating persons. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44, 252272. DOI 10.1007/s12124–010–9126–7.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. & Varela, F. (1992). The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding. Boston: Shambhala Publications.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (2006). Laws of Seeing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (2008). Gesetze der Sehens. Eschborn, Germany: Dietmar Klotz.Google Scholar
Ong, W. (1982). Orality and Literacy: The Technologizing of the World. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1966). L'image mental chez l'enfant. Paris: Press Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Premack, D. G. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.Google Scholar
Rivière, A. & Sotillo, M. (1999). Comunicazione, suspensione e semiosi umana: Le origine edella prattica e de la comprensione interpersonale. Metis, 1, 4572.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2007a). Acts of psyche: Actuations as synthesis of semiosis and action. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 205237). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2007b). Dramaturgical actuations and symbolic communication: Or how beliefs make up reality. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 293317). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2015). The reflective mind and reflexivity in psychology: Description and explanation within a psychology of experience. In Marsico, G., Ruggieri, R., & Salvatore, S. (Eds.), Reflexivity and Psychology (Yearbook of Idiographic Science, vol. 6, pp. 1744). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2016). The self rises up from lived experiences: A micro-semiotic analysis of the unfolding of trajectories of experience when performing ethics. In Valsiner, J., Marsico, G., Chaudhary, N., Sato, T., & Dazzani, V. (Eds.), Psychology as the Science of Human Being: The Yokohama Manifesto (pp. 105127). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E. & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573605.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145172. DOI: 10.1037/0033–295X.110.1.145.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Valsiner, J. (2010). Between the general and the unique: Overcoming the nomothetic versus idiographic opposition. Theory & Psychology, 20(6), 817833.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2004). Feelings integrate the central representation of appraisal-driven response organization in emotion. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N. & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and Emotions (pp. 136157). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonesson, G. (1994). The concept of text in cultural semiotics. Communication to the 3rd Congress of the Nordic Association for Semiotic Studies, Trodheim. Retrieved from www.academia.edu/5424645/GÖRAN_SONESSON_The_Concept_of_Text_in_Cultural_Semiotics_1_The_Concept_of_Text_in_Cultural_Semiotics.Google Scholar
Sonesson, G. (2010). Here comes the semiotic species: Reflections on the semiotic turn in the cognitive sciences. In Wagoneer, B. (ed.), Symbolic Transformation: The Mind in Movement through Culture and Society (pp. 3858). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Steels, L. (1996). Emergent adaptive lexicons. In Maes, P. (Ed.), From Animals To Animats: Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Simulating Adaptive Behavior (vol. 4). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Steels, L. & McIntyre, A. (1999). Spatially distributed naming games. Advances in Complex Systems, 1(4), 301323.Google Scholar
Stjernfeldt, F. (2014). Natural Propositions: The Actuality of Peirce's Doctrine of Dicisigns. Boston: Docent Press.Google Scholar
Toomela, A. (2015). Vygotsky's theory on the Procrustes’ bed of linear thinking: Looking for structural–systemic Theseus to save the idea of “social formation of mind.” Culture & Psychology, 21: 318339. DOI: 10.1177/1354067X15570490.Google Scholar
Toulmin, S. & Goodfield, J. (1965). The Discovery of Time. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2002). Irreversibility of time and ontopotentiality of signs. Estudios de Psicologia, 23(1), 4959.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2009). Cultural psychology today: Innovations and oversights. Culture & Psychology, 15(1), 539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2012). A Guided Science: History of Psychology in the Mirror of its Making. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2014). Invitation to Cultural Psychology. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2015). The place for synthesis: Vygotsky's analysis of affective generalization. History of the Human Sciences, 28(2), 93102.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2016). The nomothetic function of the idiographic approach: Looking from inside out. Journal of Person-Oriented Research, 2(1–2), 515. DOI: 10.17505/jpor:2016.02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (2007). The myth, and beyond: Ontology of psyche and epistemology of psychology. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 2339). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J. & Shear, J. (1999). First-person methodologies: What, why, how? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(12), 114.Google Scholar
Vogt, P. (2002). The physical symbol grounding problem. Cognitive Systems Research, 3, 429457.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, (1925/1971). Psychology of Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wagoner, B. (2017). Frederic Bartlett – A Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. M. (1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: W. Engelman.Google Scholar
Ziemke, T. & Sharkey, N. E. (2001). A stroll through the worlds of robots and animals: Applying Jakob von Uexküll's theory of meaning to adaptive robots and artificial life. Semiotica, 134(1–4), 701746.Google Scholar

Further Reading

Braudel, F. (1969). Écrits sur l'histoire. Paris: Flammarion.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. (2004). Emotions and action. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and Emotions (pp. 158173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335346.Google Scholar
Harris, M. (1979). Cultural Materialism: The Struggle for a Science of Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (1990). Cultura y explosión: lo previsible y lo imprevisible en los procesos de cambio social. Barcelona: Gedisa.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3:417424.Google Scholar
Sun, R. (2000). Symbol grounding: A new look at an old idea. Philosophical Psychology, 13(2), 149172.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1998). The Guided Mind: A Sociogenetic Approach to Personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

References

Atmanspacher, H. & Martin, J. (2004). Reflections on process and persons. In Weber, M. (Ed.), After Whitehead: Rescher on Process Metaphysics (pp. 161172). Frankfurt: Ontos.Google Scholar
Baerveldt, C. & Verheggen, T. (2012). Enactivism. In Valsiner, J. (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Culture and Psychology (pp. 165190). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, E. (1991). Symbolic Action Theory and Cultural Psychology. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and Cognitive Science. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bühler, K. (1934/1990). Theory of Language: The Representational Function of Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Feldman-Barrett, L. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 2046.Google Scholar
Green, C. D. (2015). Why psychology isn't unified, and probably never will be. Review of General Psychology, 19(3), 207214.Google Scholar
Heft, H. (2013). Environment, cognition, and culture: Reconsidering the cognitive map. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 33, 1425.Google Scholar
Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven Psychologies. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Heine, S. (2011). Cultural Psychology. San Francisco: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Henriques, G. (2011). A New Unified Theory of Psychology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Kimble, G. A. (1990). Mother Nature's bag of tricks is small. Psychological Science, 1, 3641.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The use of knowledge in discourse processing: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163182.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K. & Dumais, S. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211240.Google Scholar
Lickliter, R. & Honeycutt, H. (2013). A developmental evolutionary framework for psychology. Review of General Psychology, 17, 184189. DOI: 10.1037/a0032932.Google Scholar
Linell, P. (2009). Rethinking Language, Mind and World Dialogically: Interactional and Contextual Theories of Sense-making. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (2011). From association to organization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 232235. DOI: 10.1177/0963721411414656.Google Scholar
Marsh, T. & Boag, S. (2014). Unifying psychology: Shared ontology and the continuum of practical assumptions. Review of General Psychology, 18, 4959.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (Ed.). (2001). The Handbook of Culture and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maturana, M. R., & Varela, J. F. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Reidel.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1897/1932). Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (ed. by Hartshorne, C. & Weiss, P., vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. (2012). Social life of the sign: Sensemaking in society. In Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Culture and Psychology (pp. 241254). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. (2016). Cultural psychology of desire. In Valsiner, J., Marsico, G., Chaudhary, N., Sato, T., & Dazzani, V. (Eds.), Psychology as the Science of Human Being: The Yokohama Manifesto (pp. 3349). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. (2016). Psychology in Black and White: The Project for a Theory Driven Science. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S., Forges Davanzati, G., Potì, S., & Ruggieri, R. (2009). Mainstream economics and sensemaking. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 43(2), 158177.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Freda, M. F. (2011). Affect unconscious and sensemaking: A psychodynamic semiotic and dialogic model. New Ideas in Psychology, 29, 119135.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S., Gelo, O. G., Gennaro, A., Metrangolo, R., Terrone, G., Pace, V., Venuleo, C., & Venezia, A. (2015). An automated method of content analysis for psychotherapy research: A further validation. Psychotherapy Research, 25(4), 113.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Gennaro, A. (2012). The inherent dialogicality of the clinical exchange. International Journal for Dialogical Science, 6(1), 114.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S., Tebaldi, C., & Potì, S. (2006/2009). The discursive dynamic of sensemaking. In Salvatore, S., Valsiner, J., Strout, S., & Clegg, J. (Eds.), Yearbook of Idiographic Science (vol. 1, pp. 3972). Rome: Firera.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Tschacher, W. (2012). Time dependency of psychotherapeutic exchanges: The contribution of the theory of dynamic systems in analyzing process. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 253. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00253.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Valsiner, J. (2010). Between the general and the unique: Overcoming the nomothetic versus idiographic opposition. Theory & Psychology, 20(6), 817833.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Venuleo, C. (2013). Field and dynamic nature of sensemaking: Theoretical and methodological implications. Papers on Social Representation, 22(2), 21.1–21.41.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Zittoun, T. (2011). Outlines of a psychoanalytically informed cultural psychology. In Salvatore, S. & Zittoun, T. (Eds.), Cultural Psychology and Psychoanalysis in Dialogue: Issues for Constructive Theoretical and Methodological Synergies (pp. 346). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Smedslund, J. (1988). Psycho-Logic. Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Smedslund, J. (1995). Psychologic: Common sense and the pseudoempirical. In Smith, J. A., Harré, R., & van Langenhove, L. (Eds.), Rethinking Psychology (pp. 196206). London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Stam, H. J. (2004). Unifying psychology: Epistemological act or disciplinary maneuver? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 12591262. DOI: 10.1002/jclp.20069.Google Scholar
Uher, J. (2014). Interpreting “personality” taxonomies: Why previous models cannot capture individual-specific experiencing, behaviour, functioning and development. Major taxonomic tasks still lay ahead. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 49(4), 600655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uher, J., Addessi, E., & Visalberghi, E. (2013). Contextualised behavioural measurements of personality differences obtained in behavioural tests and social observations in adult capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 427444.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2001). Processes structure of semiotic mediaton in human development. Human Development, 44, 8497.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in Minds and Societies: Foundations of Cultural Psychology. New Delhi: SAGE.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2009). Integrating psychology within the globalizing world: A requiem to the post-modernist experiment with Wissenschaft. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 43(1), 121.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (Ed.). (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Culture and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, F., & Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Further Reading

Salvatore, S. & Valsiner, J. (2011). Idiographic science as a non-existing object: The importance of the reality of the dynamic system. In Salvatore, S., Valsiner, J., Gennaro, A., & Simon, J. B. Traves (Eds.), Yearbook of Idiographic Science (vol. 3, pp. 726). Rome: Firera.Google Scholar

References

Asch, S. E. (1987). Social Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, M. W. (2015). On (social) representations and the iconoclastic impetus. In Sammut, G., Andreouli, E., Gaskell, G., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations (pp. 4363). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, M. W. & Gaskell, G. (2008). Social representations theory: A progressive research programme for social psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 38(4), 335353.Google Scholar
Coelho, N. E. & Figueiredo, L. C. (2003). Patterns of intersubjectivity in the constitution of subjectivity: Dimensions of otherness. Culture and Psychology, 9, 193208.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2013). Things that help make us what we are. In Sammut, G., Daanen, P., & Moghaddam, F. M. (Eds.), Understanding the Self and Others: Explorations in Intersubjectivity and Interobjectivity (pp. 6676). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Daanen, P. & Sammut, G. (2012). G. H. Mead and knowing how to act: Practical meaning, routine interaction and the theory of interobjectivity. Theory and Psychology, 22(5), 556571.Google Scholar
Harré, R. & Sammut, G. (2013). What lies between? In Sammut, G., Daanen, P., & Moghaddam, F. M. (Eds.), Understanding the Self and Others: Explorations in Intersubjectivity and Interobjectivity (pp. 1530). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jovchelovitch, S. (2007). Knowledge in Context: Representations, Community and Culture. Hove, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jovchelovitch, S. & Priego-Hernández, J. (2015). Cognitive polyphasia, knowledge encounters and public spheres. In Sammut, G., Andreouli, E., Gaskell, G., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations (pp. 163178). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kärrholm, M. (2014). Interobjectivity in architectural research and theory: Towards a meta-theory of materiality and the effects of architecture and everyday life. The Journal of Architecture, 19(1), 6480.Google Scholar
Kolakowski, L. (1972). Positivist Philosophy: From Hume to the Vienna Circle. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1996). On interobjectivity. Mind, Culture and Activity, 3(4), 228245.Google Scholar
Marsico, G., Cabell, K. R., Valsiner, J., & Kharlamov, N. A. (2013). Interobjectivity as a border: The fluid dynamics of “betweenness.” In Sammut, G., Daanen, P., & Moghaddam, F. M. (Eds.), Understanding the Self and Others: Explorations in Intersubjectivity and Interobjectivity (pp. 5165). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2003). Interobjectivity and culture. Culture & Psychology, 9(3), 221232.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2010). Intersubjectivity, interobjectivity and the embryonic fallacy in developmental science. Culture & Psychology, 16(4), 465475.Google Scholar
Ross, L. & Ward, A. (1996). Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In Brown, T., Reed, E. S., & Turiel, E. (Eds.),Values and Knowledge (pp. 103135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sammut, G. (2015). Attitudes, social representations and points of view. In Sammut, G., Andreouli, E., Gaskell, G., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations (pp. 96112). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sammut, G. & Bauer, M. (2011). Social influence: Modes and modalities. In Hook, D., Franks, B., & Bauer, M. (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Communication (pp. 87106). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sammut, G., Bezzina, F., & Sartawi, M. (2015). The spiral of conflict: Naïve realism and the black sheep effect in attributions of knowledge and ignorance. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 21(2), 289294.Google Scholar
Sammut, G., Daanen, P., & Moghaddam, F. M. (Eds.). (2013). Understanding the Self and Others: Explorations in Intersubjectivity and Interobjectivity. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sammut, G., Daanen, P., & Sartawi, M. (2010). Interobjectivity: Representations and artefacts in Cultural Psychology. Culture & Psychology, 16(4), 451463.Google Scholar
Sammut, G. & Howarth, C. (2014). Social representations. In Teo, T. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology (pp. 17991802). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Sammut, G. & Moghaddam, F. (2014). Interobjectivity. In Teo, T. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology (pp. 991993). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Sammut, G. & Sartawi, M. (2012). Perspective-taking and the attribution of ignorance. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 42(2), 181200.Google Scholar
Sandel, M. J. (2012). What Money Can't Buy: The Moral Limits of Markets. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Wagner, W. (2015). Representation in action. In Sammut, G., Andreouli, E., Gaskell, G., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations (pp. 1228). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, W., Sen, R., Permanadeli, R., & Howarth, C. (2012). The veil and Muslim women's identity: Cultural pressures and resistance to stereotyping. Culture & Psychology, 18(4), 7999.Google Scholar

References

Bentley, A. F. (1938). Physicists and fairies. Philosophy of Science, 5, 132165.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. & Terveen, L. (1995). Foundational Issues in Artificial Intelligence and Cognitive Science: Impasse and Solution. Amsterdam: Elsevier Scientific.Google Scholar
Billig, M. (1997). Discursive, rhetorical, and ideological messages. In McGarty, C. & Haslam, S. A. (Eds.), The Message of Social Psychology: Perspectives on Mind in Society (pp. 3653). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Borghi, A. M. (2005). Object concepts and action. In Pecher, D. & Zwaan, R. A. (Eds.), Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking (pp. 834). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowker, G. C., & Star, S. L. (2000). Sorting Things Out: Classification and Its Consequences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. & Hewstone, M. (2004). Introduction. In Brewer, M. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), Social Cognition (pp. xixii). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Carr, H. A. (1915). Review of J. B. Watson (1914). Behavior: an introduction to comparative psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 12, 308312.Google Scholar
Connelly, J. & Costall, A. (2000). R. G. Collingwood and the idea of an historical psychology. Theory & Psychology, 10, 147170.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (1988). A closer look at direct perception. In Gellatly, A., Rogers, D., & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.), Cognition and Social Worlds (pp. 1021). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (1990). Picture perception as “indirect” perception. In Landwehr, K. (Ed.), Ecological Perception Research, Visual Communication and Aesthetics (pp. 1522). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (1991). Graceful degradation: Cognitivism and the metaphors of the computer. In Still, A.. & Costall, A. (Eds.), Against Cognitivism (pp. 151170). London: Harvester-Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (1995). Socializing affordances. Theory and Psychology, 5, 467481.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2004). From Darwin to Watson (and cognitivism) and back again: The principle of animal-environment mutuality. Behavior & Philosophy, 32, 179195.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2006a). “Introspectionism” and the mythical origins of scientific psychology. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 634654.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2006b). On being the right size: Affordances and the question of scale. In Lock, G. & Molyneaux, B. (Eds.), Confronting Scale in Archaeology: Issues of Theory and Practice (pp. 1526). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2007). Bringing the body back to life: James Gibson's ecology of agency. In Zlatev, J., Ziemke, T., Frank, R., & Dirven, R. (Eds.), Body, Language and Mind. Vol. 1: Embodiment (pp. 241270). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2012). Introspection and the myth of methodological behaviourism. In Clegg, J. W. (Ed.), Self-observation in the Social Sciences (pp. 6780). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.Google Scholar
Costall, A. (2013). The unconscious theory in modern cognitivism. In Racine, T. P. & Slaney, K. L. (Eds.), A Wittgensteinian Perspective on the Use of Conceptual Analysis in Psychology (pp. 312327). London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Costall, A. & Dreier, O. (2006). Doing Things with Things: The Design and Use of Objects. London: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Costall, A., Leudar, I., & Reddy, V. (2006). Failing to see the irony in “mind-reading.” Theory & Psychology, 16, 163167.Google Scholar
Costall, A. P. & Still, A. W. (1989). James Gibson's theory of direct perception and the problem of cultural relativism. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 19, 433441.Google Scholar
Cronon, W. (Ed.) (1996). Uncommon ground: rethinking the human place in nature. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1997). The varieties of social construction. Theory & Psychology, 7, 399416.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1910). The influence of Darwin on philosophy. In The Influence of Darwin on Philosophy and Other Essays (pp. 119). New York: Henry Holt & Co. (First published in Popular Science Monthly, July 1909.)Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1914/1977). Psychological doctrine and philosophical teaching. In Morgenbesser, S. (Ed.), Dewey and His Critics (pp. 439445). New York: Journal of Philosophy. (First published in the Journal of Philosophy Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1914, 11(19).)Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1917). The need for a recovery of philosophy. In Dewey, J. (Ed.), Essays in the Pragmatic Attitude (pp. 369). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1958). Experience and Nature. New York: Dover. (Based on the Paul Carus lectures of 1925.)Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1969). The Quest for Certainty. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Duncker, K. (1947). Phenomenology and epistemology of consciousness of objects. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 7, 505542.Google Scholar
Edwards, D. (1995). A commentary on discursive and cultural psychology. Culture and Psychology, 1, 5565.Google Scholar
Ellen, R. (1996). Introduction. In Ellen, Roy & Fukui, Katsuyoshi (Eds.), Redefining Nature: Ecology, Culture and Domestication (pp. 136). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Falmagne, R. J. (1995). The abstract and the concrete. In Martin, L. M. W., Nelson, K., & Tobach, E. (Eds.), Sociocultural Psychology: Theory and Practice of Doing and Knowing (pp. 205228). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1981). Representations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, W. J. & Nunez, R. (2001). Restoring to cognition the forgotten primacy of action, intention and emotion. In Nunez, R. & Freeman, W. J. (Eds.), Reclaiming Cognition: The Primacy of Action, Intention and Emotion (pp. ixxix). Thoverton, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652670.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1975). The Interpretation of Cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1950). The implications of learning theory for social psychology. In Miller, J. G. (Ed.), Experiments in Social Process (pp. 120133). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1989). Dismantling reality. In Lawson, H. & Appignanesi, L. (Eds.), Dismantling Truth: Reality in the Post-modern World (pp. 93100). London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Hall, S. (1997a). Introduction. In Hall, Stuart (Ed.), Representation: Cultural Representations and Signifying Practices (pp. 111). London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Hall, S. (1997b). The work of representation. In Hall, Stuart (Ed.), Representation: Cultural Representations and Signifying Practices (pp. 1364). London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Hammond, H. & Keat, R. (1991). Understanding Phenomenology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335346.Google Scholar
Harré, R. (2002). Cognitive Science: A Philosophical Introduction. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Heft, H. (2001). Ecological Psychology in Context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the Legacy of William James's Radical Empiricism. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven Psychologies. New York: Century.Google Scholar
Hirsch, P. D. (2000). Beyond discipline: toward effective partnerships between conservation biologists and ecological anthropologists. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Ecology, University of Georgia.Google Scholar
Holt, E. B. (1914). The Concept of Consciousness. London: George Allen.Google Scholar
Holt, E. B., Marvin, W. T., Montague, W. P., Perry, R. B., Pitkin, W. B., & Spaulding, E. G. (1912). The New Realism: Cooperative Studies in Philosophy. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hutchby, I. (2001). Technologies, texts and affordances. Sociology, 35, 441456.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2000). The Perception of the Environment: Essays in Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Janlert, L. E. (1987). Modeling change: the frame problem. In Pylyshyn, Z. W. (Ed.), The Robot's Dilemma: The Frame Problem in Artificial Intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Joerges, B. (1988). Technology in everyday life: Conceptual queries. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour; 18, 219237.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1988). The Computer and the Mind. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Joravsky, D. (1989). Russian Psychology: A Critical History. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kessen, W. (1981). Early settlements in new cognition. Cognition, 10, 167171.Google Scholar
Kwa, C. (1987). Representations of nature mediating between ecology and science policy: The case of the International Biological Programme. Social Studies of Science, 17, 413442.Google Scholar
Leeper, R. (1951). Cognitive processes. In Stevens, S. S. (Ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology (pp. 730757). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.Google Scholar
Leudar, I. & Costall, A. (Eds.). (2004a). Theory of mind. Theory & Psychology (special issue), 14(5), 571752.Google Scholar
Leudar, I. & Costall, A. (2004b). On the persistence of the problem of other minds’ in psychology: Chomsky, Grice and “theory of mind.” Theory & Psychology, 14, 603662.Google Scholar
Leudar, I. & Costall, A. (2009). Against Theory of Mind. London: Macmillan Palgrave.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, A. O. (1929). The Revolt against Dualism: An Inquiry Concerning the Existence of Ideas. LaSalle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
MacMurray, J. (1961). Persons in Relation. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Marshall, H. H. (1996). Clarifying and implementing contemporary psychological perspectives. Educational Psychologist, 31, 2934.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1938). The Philosophy of the Act. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1975). Visual anthropology in a discipline of words. In Hoskins, P. (Ed.), Principles of Visual Anthropology (pp. 310). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Pribram, K., & Galanter, E. (1960). Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Nadler, S. (1997). Descartes's dualism? Philosophical Books, 38(3), 157169.Google Scholar
Pecher, D. & Zwaan, R. A. (Eds.). (2005). Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1986). Computation and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1988). James J. Gibson and the Psychology of Perception. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1999). Reason. Linacre Journal, 3, 7184.Google Scholar
Schiffer, M. B. (with Miller, A. R.). (1999). The Material Life of Human Beings: Artifacts, Behavior, and Communication. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shalin, D. N. (1993). Modernity, postmodernism, and pragmatist inquiry: An introduction. Symbolic Interaction, 16, 303332.Google Scholar
Shaw, R. E. (2003). The agent–environment interface: Simon's indirect or Gibson's direct coupling. Ecological Psychology, 15, 37106.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. & Sullivan, M. A. (1990). The semiotic subject of cultural psychology. In Previn, L. A. (Ed.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 399416). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R. & MacKie, D. M. (2000). Social Psychology (2nd edn.). London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stevens, S. (Ed.). (1997). Conservation through Cultural Survival: Indigenous Peoples and Protected Areas. Washington, DC: Island Press.Google Scholar
Still, A. & Costall, A. (1991a). The mutual elimination of dualism in Vygotsky and Gibson. In Still, A.. & Costall, A. (Eds.), Against Cognitivism (pp. 225236). London: Harvester-Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Still, A. & Costall, A. (Eds.). (1991b). Against Cognitivism. London: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Peplau, L. A., & Sears, D. O. (1994). Social Psychology (8th edn.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Thomas, J. (1999). Some problems with the notion of external symbolic storage, and the case of Neolithic material culture in Britain. In Renfrew, C., & Scarre, C. (Eds.), Cognition and Material Culture. Oxford: McDonald Institute Monographs.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1995). Foreword. In Baron-Cohen, S., Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind (pp. ixxviii). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Toulmin, S. (1976). Knowing and Acting: An Invitation to Philosophy. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. (1996). The Role of Abstract Rationality in Vygotsky's Image of Mind. In Tryphon, A. & Voneche, J. (Eds.), Piaget-Vygotsky: The Social Genesis of Thought (pp. 2543). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar

References

Abbey, E. A. (2007). Perpetual uncertainty of cultural life: Becoming reality. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 362372). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Arcoverde, R., Amazonas, M. C., & De Lima, R. (2016). Descriptions and interpretations on self-harming. Culture & Psychology, 22(1), 110127.Google Scholar
Aron, L. (1991). The patient's experience of the analyst's subjectivity. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 1, 2951.Google Scholar
Aron, L. (2001). A Meeting of Minds: Mutuality in Psychoanalysis. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Baranger, M. & Baranger, W. (1961/2008). The analytic situation as a dynamic field. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 89(4), 795826.Google Scholar
Beebe, B. & Lachmann, F. (1998). Co-constructing inner and relational process: Self and mutual regulation in infant research and adult treatment. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 15(4), 480516.Google Scholar
Beebe, B. & Lachmann, F. (2003). The relational turn in psychoanalysis: A dyadic systems view from infant research. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 39(3), 379409.Google Scholar
Benjamin, J. (1988). The Bonds of Love: Psychoanalysis, Feminism, and the Problem of Domination. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Benjamin, J. (1998). Like Subjects Love Objects: Essays on Recognition and Sexual Difference. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Benjamin, J. (2004). Beyond doer and done to: An intersubjective view of Thirdness. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 73, 546.Google Scholar
Boston Change Process Study Group (BCPSG). (2007). The foundational level of psychodynamic meaning: Implicit process in relation to conflict, defense, and the dynamic unconscious. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 88, 843860.Google Scholar
Brinkmann, S. (2014). Psychiatric diagnoses as semiotic mediators: The case of ADHD. Nordic Psychology, 66, 121134.Google Scholar
Brinkmann, S. (2016). Toward a cultural psychology of mental disorder: The case of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Culture & Psychology, 22, 8093.Google Scholar
Bromberg, P. M. (1996). Standing in the spaces: The multiplicity of self and the psychoanalytic relationship. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 32, 509535.Google Scholar
Bromberg, P. M. (1998). Standing in the Spaces. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Bromberg, P. M. (2000). Potholes on the royal road: Or is it an abyss. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 36, 528.Google Scholar
Bromberg, P. M. (2003). Something this way comes – Trauma, dissociation, and conflict: The space where psychoanalysis, cognitive science, and neuroscience overlap. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 20, 558574.Google Scholar
Bromberg, P. M. (2006). Awakening the Dreamer: Clinical Journeys. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bromberg, P. M. (2011). Shadow of the Tsunami and the Growth of the Relational Mind. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning: Four Lectures on Mind and Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Butler, J. (1990). Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cushman, P. (1995). Constructing the Self, Constructing America: A Cultural History of Psychotherapy. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Cushman, P. (2015). Relational psychoanalysis as political resistance. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 51, 423459.Google Scholar
Duveen, G. (2007). Culture and social representations. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 543559). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eagle, M. N. (2003). The postmodern turn in psychoanalysis: A critique. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 20, 411424.Google Scholar
Ehrenberg, D. (1992). The Intimate Edge: Extending the Reach of Psychoanalytic Interaction. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from Freedom. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Fromm, E. (1951). The Forgotten Language: An Introduction to the Understanding of Dreams, Fairy Tales and Myths. New York: Grove Press.Google Scholar
Fromm, E. (1956). The Art of Loving. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Fromm-Reichmann, F. (1950). Principles of Intensive Psychotherapy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. R. (2001). The analyst's participation: A new look. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 49, 359381.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. R. & Mitchell, S. A. (1983). Object Relations in Psychoanalytic Theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, A. E. (2005). Gender as Soft Assembly (Relational Perspectives series). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Harris, A. E. (2011). The relational tradition: Landscape and cannon. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 59, 701735.Google Scholar
Hart, A. (2017). From multicultural competence to radical openness: A psychoanalytic engagement of otherness. The American Psychoanalyst, 51, 1227.Google Scholar
Heft, H. (2012). Environment, cognition and culture: Reconsidering the cognitive map. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 33, 1425.Google Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M. (2001). The dialogical self: Towards a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture & Psychology, 7, 243281. DOI: 10.1177/1354067X0173001.Google Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M. & Kempen, H. J. G. (1993). The Dialogical Self: Meaning as Movement. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, I. Z. (1983). The patient as interpreter of the analyst's experience. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 19, 389322.Google Scholar
Hoffman, I. Z. (1991). Discussion: Towards a social-constructionist view of the psychoanalytic situation. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 1, 74105.Google Scholar
Horwitz, A. V. & Wakefield, J. C. (2007). The Loss of Sadness: How Psychiatry Transformed Normal Sorrow into Depressive Disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890/1950). Principles of Psychology (vols. I and II). New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Lehman, O. V. (2014). Towards dialogues with and within silent psychotherapy sessions: Why the person of the therapist and the client matters. Culture & Psychology, 20, 537546.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (1979). Language and healing. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry, 7, 271282.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (1979/2005). The Fallacy of Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (1982). Follow the fox: An inquiry into the vicissitudes of psychoanalytic supervision. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 18, 115.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (1983/2005). The Ambiguity of Change. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (1989). Whatever happened to the cat? Interpersonal perspectives on the self. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 25, 537553.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (1991). The Purloined Self: Interpersonal Perspectives in Psychoanalysis. New York: Contemporary Psychoanalysis Books.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (2002). And the last shall be the first: Some observations on the evolution of interpersonal psychoanalysis. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 38, 277285.Google Scholar
Levenson, E. A. (2003). On seeing what is said: Visual aids to the psychoanalytic process. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 39, 233249.Google Scholar
Marková, I. (2003a). Dialogicality and Social Representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marková, I. (2003b). Constitution of the self: Intersubjectivity and dialogicality. Culture & Psychology, 9, 249259.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1967). Mind, Self and Society: From the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist (Works of George Herbert Mead, vol. 1). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mills, J. (2011). Conundrums: A Critique of Relational Psychoanalysis. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. A. (1988). Relational Concepts in Psychoanalysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. A. (1995). Hope and Dread in Psychoanalysis. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. A. (1997). Influence and Autonomy in Psychoanalysis. Hillside, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Moscovici, S. (1961/2008). Psychoanalysis: Its Image and Its Public. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Orange, D. M. (2003). Why language matters to psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic dialogues, 13, 77103.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1878/1982). How to make our ideas clear. In Thayer, H. S. (Ed.), Pragmatism: The Classic Writings (pp. 79100). Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. J. (2015a). Introduction: The postmodern turn in relational psychoanalysis. In Rosenbaum, P. (Ed.). Making Our Ideas Clear: Pragmatism and Psychoanalysis, Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. J. (2015b). Harry Stack Sullivan and Charles Sanders Peirce: The impact of early pragmatism on interpersonal psychoanalysis. In. Rosenbaum, P. (Ed.), Making Our Ideas Clear: Pragmatism and Psychoanalysis. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. J. (2016). Roger Bibace: An interpersonal thinker. In Valsiner, J., Watzlawik, M., & Kriebel, A. (Eds.), Particulars and Universals in Clinical and Developmental Psychology: Critical Reflections. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. J. & Liebert, H. (2015). Reframing the conversation on college student mental health. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 29, 179196.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. (2015). From meaning to sensemaking: Implications of a semiotic and dynamic model of mind for psychoanalysis. In. Rosenbaum, P. (Ed.), Making Our Ideas Clear: Pragmatism and Psychoanalysis. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. (2016). Psychology in Black and White: The Project of a Theory-Driven Science. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Stern, D. B. (2004). The eye sees itself: Dissociation enactment and the achievement of conflict. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 40, 197237.Google Scholar
Stern, D. B. (2003). Unformulated Experience: From Dissociation to Imagination in Psychoanalysis. Hillside, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D. B. (2009). Partners in Thought: Working with Unformulated Experience, Dissociation and Enactment (Psychoanalysis in a New Key Book Series). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Stern, D. B. (2013). Field theory in psychoanalysis, Part I: Harry Stack Sullivan and Madeleine and Willy Baranger. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 23(5), 487501.Google Scholar
Stern, D. B. (2015). Relational Freedom: Emergent Properties of the Interpersonal Fields (Psychoanalysis in a New Key Book Series). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Stern, D. B., Mann, C. H., Kantor, S., & Schlesinger, G. (Eds.). (1995). Pioneers of Interpersonal Psychoanalysis. London: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Sullivan, H. S. S. (1938/1995). Data of psychiatry. In Stern, D. B., Mann, C. H., Kantor, S., & Schlesinger, G. (Eds.), Pioneers of Interpersonal Psychoanalysis (pp. 126). London: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Sullivan, H. S. S. (1950). The illusion of individual personality. Psychiatry, 13, 317332.Google Scholar
Sullivan, H. S. S. (1953a). Conceptions of Modern Psychiatry. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Sullivan, H. S. S. (1953b). The Interpersonal Theory of Psychiatry. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Sullivan, H. S. S. (1953c). The Psychiatric Interview. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. (2005). Dynamic systems theory and the complexity of change. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 15, 255283.Google Scholar
Thompson, C. (1950/2003). Psychoanalysis: Evolution and Development. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Tublin, S. (2011). Discipline and freedom in relational technique. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 47, 519546.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2006). The semiotic construction of solitude. Sign Systems Studies, 34, 934.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in Minds and Societies: Foundations of Cultural Psychology. New York: SAGE.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2009). Cultural psychology today: Innovations and oversights. Culture & Psychology, 15, 539.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2014). An Invitation to Cultural Psychology. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Wagner, W., Duveen, G., Verma, J., & Themel, M. (2000). “I have some faith and at the same time I don't believe in it” – Cognitive polyphasia and culture change in India. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 10, 301314.Google Scholar
Wagner, W., Sen, R., Permanadeli, R., & Howarth, C. S. (2012). The veil and Muslim women's identity: Cultural pressures and resistances to stereotyping. Culture & Psychology, 18, 521541.Google Scholar

Further Reading

Blechner, M. J. (2005). The gay Harry Stack Sullivan: Interactions between his life, clinical work and theory. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 41(1), 119.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1995). Culture and cognitive development: From cross-cultural research to creating systems of cultural mediation. Culture & Psychology, 1, 2554.Google Scholar
Harre, R. (2015). The persons as the nexus of patterns of discursive practices. Culture & Psychology, 21, 492504.Google Scholar
Marsico, G. (2015). Striving for the new: Cultural psychology as a developmental science. Culture & Psychology, 21, 445454.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2, 201218.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P. J. (2013). The role of projective identification in constructing the “Other”: Why do Westerners want to “liberate” Muslim women? Culture & Psychology 19(2), 213224. DOI: 10.1177/1354067X12456719.Google Scholar
Shewder, R. (1990). Thinking Through Cultures: Expeditions in Cultural Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2015). Where are you, Culture & Psychology? Making of an interdisciplinary field. Culture & Psychology, 21, 419428.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (2007). Contemporary socio-cultural research: Uniting culture, society and psychology. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 122). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., Duveen, G., Gillespie, A., Ivinson, G., & Psaltis, C. (2003). The use of symbolic resources in developmental transitions. Culture & Psychology, 9, 415448.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T. & Gillespie, A. (2015). Internalization: How culture becomes mind. Culture & Psychology, 21, 477491.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×