Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T22:03:12.687Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 May 2019

Jennifer Smith
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Mercedes Durham
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Sociolinguistic Variation in Children's Language
Acquiring Community Norms
, pp. 200 - 216
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adger, David 2006. Combinatorial variability. Journal of Linguistics, 42 (3), 503–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adger, David 2014. Variability and grammatical architecture. In Picallo, C. (ed.) Variation in a Minimalist Perspective, 179–96. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adger, David 2016. Language variability in syntactic theory. In Mendikoetxea, A. (ed.) Rethinking Parameters, 4963. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adger, David and Smith, Jennifer 2005. Variation and the minimalist program. In Cornips, Leonie and Corrigan, Karen P. (eds), Syntax and Variation: Reconciling the Biological and the Social, 149–78. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Adger, David and Smith, Jennifer 2010. Variation in agreement: a lexical feature-based approach. Lingua 120 (5), 1109–34.Google Scholar
Aitchison, Jean 2012. Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon, 4th edn. Oxford and Cambridge: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Aitken, Adam J. and Macafee, Caroline (eds) 2002. The Older Scots Vowels: A History of the Stressed Vowels of Older Scots from the Beginnings to the Eighteenth Century. Edinburgh: Scottish Text Society.Google Scholar
Albin, Drema D. and Echols, Catharine H. 1996. Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 19 (4), 401–18.Google Scholar
Anderson, Elaine 1990. Speaking with Style: The Sociolinguistic Skills of Children. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Anderwald, Lieselotte 2004. The morphology and syntax of the varieties of English spoken in the Southeast of England. In Kortmann, Bernd, Schneider, Edgar W., Burridge, Kate, Mesthrie, Rajend and Upton, Clive (eds), A Handbook of Varieties of English, Vol. II, 157–78. Berlin/New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anderwald, Lieselotte 2004. The varieties of English in the Southeast of England: morphology and syntax. In Kortmann, Bernd and Upton, Clive (eds), Varieties of English I: The British Isles, 373403. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anthony, Laurence 2016. AntConc (version 3.2. 4)[computer software]. Tokyo: Waseda University.Google Scholar
Ash, Sharon and Myhill, John 1986. Linguistic correlates of inter-ethnic contact. In Sankoff, David (ed.), Diversity and Diachrony, 3344. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Austin, Alison C., Newport, Elissa L. and Wonnacott, Elizabeth 2006. Predictable versus unpredictable variation: regularization in adult and child learners. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Child Language Development (BUCLD), Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Bann, Jennifer and Corbett, John 2015. Spelling Scots: The Orthography of Literary Scots, 1700–2000. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, Dale, Philip S. and Thal, Donna 1995. Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In Fletcher, Paul and MacWhinney, Brian (eds), The Handbook of Child Language, 96151. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bayley, Robert 1994. Consonant cluster reduction in Tejano English. Language Variation and Change, 6 (3), 303–26.Google Scholar
Beal, Joan and Corrigan, Karen 2002. Relatives in Tyneside and Northumbrian English. In Poussa, P. (ed.), Relativisation on the North Sea Littoral. Munich, Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Bell, Allan 1984. Language style as audience design. Language in Society, 13 (2), 145204.Google Scholar
Bickerton, Derek 1975. Dynamics of a Creole System. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Biemiller, Andrew 1999. Language and Reading Success. Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books.Google Scholar
Blaikie, And rew. 2002. Coastal communities in Victorian Scotland: what makes north-east fisher families distinctive? Local Population Studies, 69, 1531.Google Scholar
Block, Jeanne H. 1979. Another look at sex differentiation in the socialization behaviors of mothers and fathers. In Denmark, Florence L. and Sherman, Julia (eds), Psychology of Women: Future Directions for Research, 2987. New York, NY: Psychological Dimensions.Google Scholar
Bornstein, Marc H., Cote, Linda R., Maital, Sharone, Painter, Kathleen, Park, Sung-Yun, Pascual, Liliana, Pêcheux, Marie-Germaine, Ruel, Josette, Venuti, Paola and Vyt, re 2004. Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development, 75 (4), 267304.Google Scholar
Britain, David 2003. Exploring the importance of the outlier in sociolinguistic dialectology. In Britain, David and Cheshire, Jenny (eds), Social Dialectology, 191208. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Broen, Patricia A. 1972. The Verbal Environment of the Language-Learning Child. ASHA Monographs 17. Washington, DC: American Speech and Hearing Association.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger 1958. Words and Things. New York, NY: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger 1973. A First Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger and Hanlon, C. 1970. Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In Hayes, J. (ed.), Cognition and the Development of Language, 1153. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Burroughs, Elizabeth I. and Murray, Sharon E. 1992. The influence of play material on discourse during play. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 14 (2), 119–28.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, Thea, Lieven, Elena and Theakston, Anna 2007. What part of no do children not understand? A usage-based account of multiword negation. Journal of Child Language, 34 (2), 251–82.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, Thea, Lieven, Elena and Tomasello, Michael 2003. A construction-based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27 (6), 846–73.Google Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn 2005. Listener Perceptions of Sociolinguistic Variables: The Case of (ING). Unpublished PhD thesis, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Chabanal, Damien 2001. L’acquisition de phonèmes variables: influences orthographique et sociolectale. Travaux de didactique du français langue étrangère 45, 8796.Google Scholar
Chambers, Jack K. 1992. Dialect acquisition. Language 68 (4), 683705.Google Scholar
Chambers, Jack K. 2002. Studying language variation: an informal epistemology. In Chambers, Jack K., Trudgill, Peter and Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 314. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chambers, Jack K. 2003. Sociolinguistic Theory. 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chambers, Jack K. (2004). Dynamic Typology and Vernacular Universals in B. Kortmann (ed.), Dialectology Meets Typology, 127145. Berlin/New york: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny 1978. Present tense verbs in Reading English. In Trudgill, Peter (ed.), Sociolinguistic Patterns in British English, 5268. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny 1981. Variation in the use of ain’t in an urban British dialect. Language in Society, 10 (3), 365–88. Reprinted 1991 in Peter Trudgill and Jack K. Chambers (eds), Dialects of English: Studies in Grammatical Variation, 54–73. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny 1982. Variation in an English Dialect: A Sociolinguistic Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny 1997. Involvement in ‘standard’ and ‘non-standard’ English. In Cheshire, J. and Stein, D. (eds), Taming the Vernacular: From Dialect to Written Standard Language, 6383. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny 1999. Spoken Standard English. In Bex, Tony and Watts, Richard J. (eds), Standard English: The Widening Debate, 129–48. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Chevrot, Jean-Pierre and Foulkes, Paul 2013. Introduction: language acquisition and sociolinguistic variation. Linguistics, 51 (2), 251–4.Google Scholar
Chevrot, Jean-Pierre, Beaud, Laurence and Varga, Renata 2000. Developmental data on a French sociolinguistic variable: post-consonantal word-final /R/. Language Variation and Change, 12 (3), 295319.Google Scholar
Chevrot, Jean-Pierre, Nardy, Aurélie and Barbu, Stéphanie 2011. Developmental dynamics of SES-related differences in children’s production of obligatory and variable phonological alternations. Language Sciences, 33 (1), 180–91.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1957. Syntactic Structures. Janua Linguarum 4. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1988. Language and Problems of Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1995. The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 2016. First Language Acquisition, 3rd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V. and Bowerman, Melissa. 1986. On the acquisition of final voiced stops. In Fishman, J. A. (ed.), The Fergusonian Impact: In Honor of Charles A. Ferguson on the Occasion of His 65th Birthday. Vol. I: From Phonology to Society, 5168. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Corbett, John B. and Stuart-Smith, Jane 2012. Standard English in Scotland. In Hickey, Raymond (ed.), Standards of English: Codified Standards Around the World. Studies in English Language, 7295. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cornips, Leonie 2017. Child acquisition of sociolinguistic variation: adults, children and (regional) standard Dutch two-verb clusters in one community. In De Vogelaer, Gunther and Katerbow, Matthias (eds), Acquiring Sociolinguistic Variation, 91116. London, John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Corsaro, William A. 1979. Young children’s conception of status and role. Sociology of Education 52, 4659.Google Scholar
Craig, Holly K. and Washington, Julie A. 2006. Malik Goes to School: Examining the Language Skills of African-American Students from Preschool–5th Grade. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crawford, William J. 2005. Verb agreement and disagreement: a corpus investigation of concord variation in existential there + be constructions. Journal of English Linguistics, 33 (1), 3561.Google Scholar
Crain, Stephen 1992. Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14 (4), 597650.Google Scholar
Cross, Toni G. 1977. Mothers’ speech adjustments: the contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, Catherine E. and Ferguson, Charles A. (eds), Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, 151–80. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cukor-Avila, Patricia 1995. The Evolution of AAVE in a Rural Texas Community: An Ethnolinguistic Study. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Cukor-Avila, Patricia and Bailey, Guy 2015. Rural Texas African American Vernacular English. In Lanehart, Sonja (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of African American Language, 181200. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dapretto, Mirella and Bjork, Elizabeth 2000. The development of word retrieval abilities in the second year and its relation to early vocabulary growth. Child Development, 71 (3), 635–48.Google Scholar
de Haas, Nynke 2011. Morphosyntactic Variation in Northern English: The Northern Subject Rule, Its Origins and Early History. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Radboud Univeristeit, Nijmegen.Google Scholar
de Haas, Nynke and Ans, van Kemenade 2015. The origin of the Northern Subject Rule: subject positions and verbal morphosyntax in older English. English Language and Linguistics, 19 (1), 4981.Google Scholar
de Villiers, Jill and de Villiers, Peter 1973. A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2 (3), 267–78.Google Scholar
De Vogelaer, Gunther, Chevrot, Jean-Pierre, Katerbow, Matthias and Nardy, Aurélie 2017. Bridging the gap between language acquisition and sociolinguistics: introduction to an interdisciplinary topic. In De Vogelaer, Gunther and Katerbow, Matthias (eds), Acquiring Sociolinguistic Variation, 142. London, John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, Manuel 2001. Acquisition of Phonological Structure and Sociolinguistic Variables: A Quantitative Analysis of Spanish Consonant Weakening in Venezuelan Children’s Speech. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, Manuel 2005. The emergence of adult-like command of sociolinguistic variables: a study of consonant weakening in Spanish-speaking children. In Eddington, David (ed.), Studies in the Acquisition of the Hispanic Languages: Papers from the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages, 5665. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Dieth, Eugen 1932. A Grammar of the Buchan Dialect. Cambridge: Heffer & Sons.Google Scholar
Docherty, Gerard J. and Foulkes, Paul 1999. Newcastle upon Tyne and Derby: instrumental phonetics and variationist studies. In Foulkes, Paul and Docherty, Gerard J. (eds), Urban Voices: Accent Studies in the British Isles, 4771. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Docherty, Gerard J., Foulkes, Paul, Dodd, Barbara and Milroy, Lesley 2002. The Emergence of Structured Variation in the Speech of Tyneside Infants. Final report to the United Kingdom Economic and Social Research Council, grant R000 237417.Google Scholar
Docherty, Gerard J., Foulkes, Paul, Milroy, Lesley and Milroy, James 1997. Descriptive adequacy in phonology: a variationist perspective. Journal of Linguistics, 33 (2), 275310.Google Scholar
Docherty, Gerard J., Foulkes, Paul, Tillotson, Jenny and Watt, Dominic 2006. On the scope of phonological learning: issues arising from socially-structured variation. In Goldstein, Louis, Whalen, Douglas H. and Best, Catherine T. (eds), Laboratory Phonology, 8, 393421. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Donegan, Patricia 2002. Normal vowel development. In Ball, Martin J. and Gibbon, Fiona E. (eds), Vowels and Vowel Disorders: A Handbook, 136. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope 2000. Linguistic Variation as Social Practice. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Edwards, Vivian 1993. The grammar of southern British English. In Milroy, James and Milroy, Lesley (eds), Real English, 214–34. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Eisikovits, Edina 1991. Variation in subject–verb agreement in inner Sydney English. In Cheshire, Jenny (ed.), English around the World: Sociolinguistic Perspectives, 235–56. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eremeeva, Viktoria and Stuart-Smith, Jane 2003. A sociophonetic investigation of the vowels OUT and BIT in Glaswegian. In Solé, Maria-Josep, Recasens, Daniel and Romero, Joaquin (eds), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences: 3–9 August 2003, Barcelona, Spain, 1205–8. Rundle Mall, Australia: Causal Productions.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan M. 1979. Children’s verbal turn-taking. In Ochs, Elinor and Schieffelin, Bambi B. (eds), Developmental Pragmatics, 391414. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fabricius, Anne 2000. T-Glottalling between Stigma and Prestige: A Sociolinguistic Study of Modern RP. Unpublished PhD thesis, Copenhagen Business School.Google Scholar
Fabricius, Anne 2002. Ongoing change in modern RP: evidence for the disappearing stigma of t-glottalling. English World-Wide, 23 (1), 115–36.Google Scholar
Fagot, Beverly and Leinbach, Mary 1987. Socialization of sex roles within the family. In Carter, D. B. (ed.), Current Conceptions of Sex Roles and Sex Typing: Theory and Research, 89100. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
Fagot, Beverly I. and Kavanagh, Kate 1993. Parenting during the second year: effects of children’s age, sex, and attachment classification. Child Development, 64 (1), 258–71.Google Scholar
Fasold, Ralph W. 1972. Tense Marking in Black English. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Feagin, Crawford 1979. Variation and Change in Alabama English: A Sociolinguistic Study of the White Community. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A. 1977. Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, Catherine E. and Ferguson, Charles A. (eds), Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, 209–35. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, Anne and Simon, Thomas 1984. Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20 (1), 104–13.Google Scholar
Filipi, Anna 2009. Toddler and Parent Interaction: The Organisation of Gaze, Pointing and Vocalisation. London, John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fischer, John L. 1958. Social influences on the choice of a linguistic variant. Word, 14 (1), 4756.Google Scholar
Flynn, Nicholas 2012. A Sociophonetic Study of Nottingham Speakers. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of York.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerard J. and Watt, Dominic 1999. Tracking the emergence of structured variation: Realisations of (t) by Newcastle children. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics, 7, 125.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerard J. and Watt, Dominic 2001. The emergence of structured variation. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 73, 6784.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerard J. and Watt, Dominic 2005. Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language, 81 (1), 77206.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul and Docherty, Gerard J. (eds) 1999. Urban Voices: Accent Studies in the British Isles. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Galasso, Joseph 2004. The acquisition of inflection and the dual mechanism model: a case study. Proceedings of the 2004 Child Language Research Forum Stanford, 98108.Google Scholar
Ganger, Jennifer and Brent, Michael 2004. Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Developmental Psychology, 40 (4), 621–32.Google Scholar
Garnica, Olga 1977. Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, Catherine E. and Ferguson, Charles A. (eds), Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, 6388. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giles, Howard 1977. Social psychology and applied linguistics: towards an integrative approach. ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 2742.Google Scholar
Gleason, Jean B. 1987. Sex differences in parent–child interaction. In Philips, Susan U., Steele, Susan and Tanz, Christine (eds), Language, Gender, and Sex in Comparative Perspective 189–199. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gleason, Jean B. and Weintraub, Sandra 1978. Input language and the acquisition of communicative competence. In Nelson, Keith E. (ed.), Children’s Language, Vol. I, 171222. New York, NY: Gardiner Press.Google Scholar
Gleason, Jean B., Perlmann, Rivka Y., Ely, Richard and Evans, David A. 1994. The babytalk register: parents’ use of diminutives. In Sokolov, Jeffrey L. and Snow, Catherine E. (eds), Handbook of Research in Language Development Using CHILDES. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Godfrey, Elizabeth and Tagliamonte, Sali A. 1999. Another piece for the verbal -s story: evidence from Devon in southwest England. Language Variation and Change, 11 (1), 87121.Google Scholar
Goodman, Judith C. and Dale, Philip S. 2008. Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 35 (3), 515–31.Google Scholar
Gordon, John (ed.) 1845. The New Statistical Account of Scotland/By the Ministers of the Respective Parishes, under the Superintendence of a Committee of the Society for the Benefit of the Sons and Daughters of the Clergy. Rathven, Banff, Vol. XIII. Edinburgh: Blackwoods and Sons.Google Scholar
Green, Lisa J. 2011. Language and the African American Child. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grunwell, Pamela. 1981. The development of phonology: a descriptive profile. First Language, 3 (6), 161–91.Google Scholar
Grunwell, Pamela 1997. Natural phonology. In Ball, Martin and Kent, Raymond (eds), The New Phonologies: Developments in Clinical Linguistics, 3575. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.Google Scholar
Guy, Gregory 1980. Variation in the group and the individual: the case of final stop deletion. In Labov, William (ed.), Locating Language in Time and Space, 136. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Guy, Gregory and Boyd, Sally 1990. The development of a morphological class. Language Variation and Change, 2 (1), 118.Google Scholar
Habib, Rania 2017. Parents and their children’s variable language: is it acquisition or more? Journal of Child Language, 44 (3), 628–49.Google Scholar
Hall, William S. and Tirre, William C. 1979. The Communicative Environment of Young Children: Social Class, Ethnic and Situational Differences. Champagne, IL: University of Illinois.Google Scholar
Harris, Tony and Wexler, Ken 1996. The optional infinitive stage in child English: evidence from negation. In Clahsen, H. (ed.), Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition: Empirical Findings, Theoretical Considerations and Crosslinguistic Comparisons. Amsterdam, John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer and Schreier, Daniel 2004. Reversing the trajectory of language change: subject–verb agreement with be in New Zealand English. Language Variation and Change, 16 (3), 209–35.Google Scholar
Henry, Alison 2016. Acquiring language from variable input: subject–verb agreement and negative concord in Belfast English. Linguistic Variation 16 (1), 131–50.Google Scholar
Henry, Alison, MacLaren, Rose, Wilson, John and Finlay, Cathy, 1997. The acquisition of negative concord in non-standard English. In Hughes, E., Hughes, M. and Greenhill, A. (eds), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hoyle, Susan and Adger, Carolyn Temple 1998. Kids Talk: Strategic Language Use in Later Childhood. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Horvath, Barbara and Horvath, Ronald 2002. The geolinguistics of/l/vocalization in Australia and New Zealand. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 6 (3), 319–46.Google Scholar
Houston, Ann C. 1985. Continuity and Change in English Morphology: The Variable (ING). Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Hudson Kam, Carla L. 2015. The impact of conditioning variables on the acquisition of variation in adult and child learners. Language, 91 (4), 906–37.Google Scholar
Hudson Kam, Carla L. and Newport, Elissa L. 2005. Regularizing unpredictable variation: the roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 1 (2), 151–95.Google Scholar
Hudson Kam, Carla L. and Newport, Elissa L. 2009. Getting it right by getting it wrong: when learners change languages. Cognitive Psychology, 59 (1), 3066.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, Janellen, Haight, Wendy, Bryk, Anthony, Seltzer, Michael and Lyons, Thomas 1991. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27 (2), 236–48.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, Janellen, Vasilyeva, Marina, Waterfall, Heidi, Vevea, Jack and Hedges, Larry V. 2007. Varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1062–83.Google Scholar
Ingram, D., Christensen, L., Veach, S. and Webster, B. 1980. The acquisition of word-initial fricatives and affricates in English by children between 2 and 6 years. In Yeni-Komshian, Grace H., Kavanagh, James F. and Ferguson, Charles A. (eds), Child Phonology Vol I: Production, 169192. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Joanne 2003. Phonetic Differences in Child-Directed Speech to Opposite Sex Twins. Unpublished BSc dissertation, University of Newcastle.Google Scholar
Johnson, Valerie E. 2005. Comprehension of third person singular /s/ in AAE-speaking children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 116–24.Google Scholar
Johnston, Paul 1997. Regional variation. In Jones, Charles (ed.), The Edinburgh History of the Scots Language, 433513. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel 1996. Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language, 72, 533–78.Google Scholar
Kaye, Kenneth 1980. Why we don’t talk ‘baby talk’ to babies. Journal of Child Language, 7 (3), 489507.Google Scholar
Keeney, Terrence J. and Wolfe, Jean 1972. The acquisition of agreement in English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11 (6), 698705.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul 1996. Children, adolescents, and language change. Language Variation and Change, 8 (2), 177202.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul 2003. Dialect levelling and geographical diffusion in British English. In Britain, David and Cheshire, Jenny (eds), Social Dialectology: In Honour of Peter Trudgill, 223–43. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul and Williams, Ann, 2000. Creating a new town koine: children and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society, 29 (1), 65115.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul and Williams, Ann 2002. ‘Salience’ as an explanatory factor in language change: evidence from dialect levelling in urban England. In Jones, Mari C. and Esch, Edith (eds), Language Change. The Interplay of Internal, External and Extra-Linguistic Factors, 81110. Berlin and New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul and Williams, Ann 2005. New towns and koineisation: linguistic and social correlates. Linguistics, 43 (5), 1023–48.Google Scholar
Kiesling, Scott F. 2009. Style as stance. In Jaffe, Alexandra (ed.), Stance: Sociolinguistic perspectives (Oxford Studies in Sociolinguistics). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kirkham, Sam and Moore, Emma 2016. Constructing social meaning in political discourse: phonetic variation and verb processes in Ed Miliband’s speeches. Language in Society, 45 (1), 87111.Google Scholar
Klee, Thomas, Schaffer, Mary, May, Susan, Membrino, Irene and Mougey, Karen 1989. A comparison of the age-MLU relation in normal and specifically language-impaired preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54 (2), 226–33.Google Scholar
Klima, Edward S. and Bellugi, Ursula 1966. Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, John and Wales, Roger J. (eds), Psycholinguistic Papers: The Proceedings of the 1966 Edinburgh Conference, 183219. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Kortmann, Bernd and Lunkenheimer, Kerstin (eds) 2013. The Electronic World Atlas of Varieties of English. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Google Scholar
Kovac, Ceil and Douglas Adamson, H. 1981. Variation theory and first language acquisition. In Sankoff, David and Cedergren, Henrietta (eds), Variation Omnibus, 403–10. Edmonton, Alberta: Linguistic Research, Inc.Google Scholar
Kushartanti, Bernadette 2014. The Acquisition of Stylistic Variation by Jakarta Indonesian Children. Utrecht, LOT.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1964. Stages in the acquisition of Standard English. In Shuy, Roger, Davis, Alva and Hogan, Robert (eds), Social Dialects and Language Learning, 77103. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1966. The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1969. Contraction, deletion and inherent variability of the English copula. Language 45, 715–62.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1984. Field methods of the project on linguistic change and variation. In Baugh, John and Sherzer, Joel (eds), Language in Use: Readings in Sociolinguistics, 2866. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1989. The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change, 1. 8597.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1990. The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Language Variation and Change, 2, 205–54.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1995. The case of the missing copula: the interpretation of zeros in African American English. In Gleitman, Lila and Liberman, Mark (eds), Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science, 2554. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Labov, William 2001. Principles of Linguistic Change: Social Factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William 2007. Transmission and diffusion. Language, 83 (2), 344–87.Google Scholar
Labov, William 2013. Preface: the acquisition of sociolinguistic variation. Linguistics, 51 (2), 247–50.Google Scholar
Labov, William, Cohen, Paul, Robins, Clarence and Lewis, John 1968. A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican Speakers in New York City. Cooperative Research Report 3288, Vols I and II. Philadelphia, PA: US Regional Survey (Linguistics Laboratory, University of Pennsylvania).Google Scholar
Labov, William and Waletzky, Joshua 1967. Narrative analysis: oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, June (ed.), Essays on the Verbal and Visual Arts, 1244. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Lacoste, Véronique and Green, Lisa 2016. Child language variation. Linguistic Variation, 16 (1), 111.Google Scholar
Lakoff, Robin 1973. Language and women’s place. Language in Society, 2 (1), 4579.Google Scholar
Lawson, Eleanor, Stuart-Smith, Jane and Scobbie, James (2008) Articulatory insights into language variation and change: preliminary findings from an ultrasound study of derhoticization in Scottish English. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 14 (2), 102110.Google Scholar
Leaper, Campbell, Anderson, Kristin J. and Sanders, Paul 1998. Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: a meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34 (1), 327.Google Scholar
Legate, Julie Anne and Yang, Charles 2007. Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense. Language Acquisition, 14, 315–44.Google Scholar
Leopold, Werner F. 1948. The study of child language and infant bilingualism. WORD, 4 (1), 117.Google Scholar
Li, Ping, Zhao, Xiaowei and MacWhinney, Brian 2007. Dynamic self-organization and early lexical development in children. Cognitive Science, 31 (4), 581612.Google Scholar
Macafee, Caroline 1983. Varieties of English Around the World: Glasgow. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Macafee, Caroline 1994. Traditional Dialect in the Modern World: A Glasgow Case Study. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Macafee, Caroline 2003. Studying Scots vocabulary. In Corbett, John B., Stuart-Smith, Jane and Derrick McClure, J. (eds), A Bief History of Scots, 5071. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Macaulay, Ronald K. S. 1977. Language, Social Class, and Education: A Glasgow Study. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Macaulay, Ronald K. S. 1991. Locating Dalect in Discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Macaulay, Ronald K. S. and Trevelyan, Gavin D. 1973. Language, Education and Employment in Glasgow. Final Report to the Social Science Research Council. Edinburgh: Scottish Council for Research in Education.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian 1987. The competition model. In MacWhinney, Brian (ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition, 249308. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian 1991. The CHILDES project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marcus, Gary, Pinker, Steven, Ullman, Michael, Hollander, Michelle, Rosen, John, Xu, Fei and Clahsen, Harald 1992. Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (4), i178.Google Scholar
Marrero, Victoria and Aguirre, Carmen 2003. Plural acquisition and development in Spanish. In Montrul, Silvina and Ordóñez, Francisco (eds), Linguistic Theory and Language Development in Hispanic Languages, 275–96. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Marshall, Jonathan 2001. The sociolinguistic status of the glottal stop in Northeast Scots. Reading Working Papers in Linguistics, 5, 4965.Google Scholar
Martin, Nathaël 2005. Réseaux sociaux et variations phonétiques. Unpublished dissertation, Université Stendhal-Grenoble III.Google Scholar
Martínez-Insua, Ana E. and Palacios Martínez, Ignacio M. 2003. A corpus-based approach to non-concord in present day English existential there-constructions. English Studies, 84 (3), 262–83.Google Scholar
Martino, Josephine 1982. The phoneme/θ/and its alternative realization as/f/: a study of variation in Australian English among primary school boys, according to socio-economic background. Working Papers in Linguistics Melbourne, 8, 3542.Google Scholar
McClure, J. Derrick 1994. English in Scotland. In Burchfield, Robert W. (ed.), The Cambridge History of the English Language, Vol. V, 2393. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McClure, J. Derrick 2003. Doric: The Dialect of North-East Scotland. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
McLeod, Sharynne, van Doorn, Jan and Reed, Vicki A. 2001a. Normal acquisition of consonant clusters. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 99110.Google Scholar
McLeod, Sharynne, van Doorn, Jan and Reed, Vicki A. 2001b. Consonant cluster development in two-year-olds: general trends and individual difference. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1144–71.Google Scholar
Meechan, Marjory and Foley, Michele 1994. On resolving disagreement: linguistic theory and variation—There’s bridges. Language Variation and Change, 6 (1), 6385.Google Scholar
Mees, Inger and Collins, Beverley 1999. Cardiff: a real-time study of glottalisation. In Foulkes, Paul and Docherty, Gerard J. (eds), Urban Voices: Accent Studies in the British Isles, 185202. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Meyerhoff, Miriam and Walker, James A. 2013. An existential problem: the sociolinguistic monitor and the variation in existential constructions on Bequia (St Vincent and the Grenadines). Language in Society, 42 (4), 407–28.Google Scholar
Millar, Robert McColl 2007. Northern and Insular Scots. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, Karen 2013. Variable input: what Sarah reveals about non-agreeing don’t and theories of root infinitives. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Language Development, 20 (4), 305–24.Google Scholar
Miller, Karen 2015. Children’s production of ain’t. In Donaher, Patricia and Katz, Seth (eds), Ain’thology: The History of Life and the Taboo Word Ain’t, 96112. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Miller, Karen and Schmitt, Cristina 2009. Variable vs consistent input: comprehension of plural morphology and verbal agreement in children. In Brucart, José M., Gavarró, Anna and Solà, Jaume (eds), Merging Features: Computation, Interpretation and Acquisition, 123–37. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, Karen and Schmitt, Cristina 2012. Variable input and the acquisition of plural morphology. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Language Development, 19 (3), 223–61.Google Scholar
Miller, Karen and Schmitt, Cristina 2014. Spanish-speaking children’s use of verbal agreement in comprehension. Lingua, 144, 4057.Google Scholar
Milroy, James and Milroy, Lesley 1985. Linguistic change, social network, and speaker innovation. Journal of Linguistics 21 (2), 339384.Google Scholar
Milroy, James, Milroy, Lesley, Hartley, Sue and Walshaw, David 1994. Glottal stops and Tyneside glottalization: competing patterns of variation and change in British English. Language Variation and Change, 6 (3), 327–57.Google Scholar
Milroy, Lesley 1980. Language and Social Networks. London and Baltimore, MD: Basil Blackwell, University Park Press.Google Scholar
Milsark, Gary L. 1977. Toward an explanation of certain peculiarities of the existential construction in English. Linguistic Analysis, 3, 130.Google Scholar
Mumsnet 2016. Priti Patel and her dropped Gs. http://www.mumsnet.com/Talk/ pedants_corner/2077339-Priti-Patel-and-her-dropped-Gs [accessed 20 June 2017].Google Scholar
Murray, James A. H. 1873. The Dialect of the Southern Counties of Scotland. London: Philological Society.Google Scholar
Nardy, Aurélie 2008. Acquisition des variables sociolinguistiques entre 2 et 6 ans: facteurs sociologiques et influences des interactions au sein du réseau social. Unpublished dissertation, Université Stendhal-Grenoble III.Google Scholar
Aurélie, Nardy, Chevrot, Jean-Pierre and Barbu, Stéphanie 2013. The acquisition of sociolinguistic variation: looking back and thinking ahead. Linguistics, 51 (2), 255–84.Google Scholar
Nardy, Aurélie, Chevrot, Jean-Pierre and Barbu, Stéphanie 2014. Sociolinguistic convergence and social interactions within a group of preschoolers: a longitudinal study. Language Variation and Change, 26 (3), 273301.Google Scholar
Nathan, Geoffrey S. 1981. What’s these facts about? Linguistic Inquiry, 1, 151–3.Google Scholar
Nelson, Katherine 1973. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (1–2), 1135.Google Scholar
Nevins, And rew and Parrott, Jeffrey 2010. Variable rules meet impoverishment theory: patterns of agreement leveling in English varieties. Lingua, 20 (5), 1135–59.Google Scholar
Newport, Elissa 1976. Motherese: the speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, N., Pisoni, D. and Potts, G. (eds), Cognitive Theory, Vol. II. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, Elissa, Gleitman, Henry and Gleitman, Lila 1977. Mother, I’d rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, Catherine E. and Ferguson, Charles A. (eds), Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, 109–50. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
O’Reilly, Anne Watson and Bornstein, Marc H. 1993. Caregiver–child interaction in play. In Bornstein, Marc H. and O’Reilly, Anne Watson (eds), The Role of Play in the Development of Thought. New Directions for Child Development, 59, 5566. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Palacios Martínez, Ignacio (2010). ‘It ain’t nothing to do with my school’. Variation and pragmatic uses of ain’t in the language of British English teenagers. English Studies, 91 (5), 548–66.Google Scholar
Patrick, Peter L. 1999. Urban Jamaican Creole: Variation in the Mesolect. Varieties of English Around the World G17. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Patterson, Janet Lee 1992. The Development of Sociolinguistic Phonological Variation Patterns for (ing) in Young Children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
Payne, Arvilla C. 1976. The Acquisition of the Phonological System of a Second Dialect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Pietsch, Lukas 2005. Variable Grammars: Verbal Agreement in Northern Dialects of English. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Phillips, Juliet R. 1973. Syntax and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. Child Development, 44, 182–5.Google Scholar
Pine, Julian M. 1994. The language of primary caregivers. In Gallaway, Clare and Richards, Brian (eds), Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition, 1537. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven 1984. Language Learnability and Language Development, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven 1989. Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven 1994. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. Harmondsworth: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Porter, Judith H. and Hodson, Barbara 2001. Collaborating to obtain phonological acquisition data for local schools. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 165–71.Google Scholar
Purcell, April 1984. Code-switching Hawaiian style: children’s accommodation along a decreolizing continuum. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 46, 7186.Google Scholar
Reid, Euan 1978. Social and stylistic variation in the speech of children: some evidence from Edinburgh. In Trudgill, Peter (ed.), Sociolinguistic Patterns in British English, 158–71. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Renn, Jennifer 2010. Acquiring Style: The Development of Dialect Shifting among African American Children. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.Google Scholar
Renn, Jennifer 2015. Investigating the Relationship between African American English Use and Early Literacy Skills. Paper presented at the American Dialect Society 2015 Annual Meeting, Portland, Oregon, January 8–11.Google Scholar
Renn, Jennifer and Terry, J. Michael 2009. Operationalizing style: quantifying style shift in the speech of African American adolescents. American Speech, 84 (4), 367–90.Google Scholar
Rice, Mabel, Smolik, Filip, Perpich, Denise, Thompson, Travis, Rytting, Nathan and Blossom, Megan 2010. Mean length of utterance levels in six-month intervals for children 3 to 9 years with and without language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53 (2), 333–49.Google Scholar
Rickford, John, Ball, Arnetha, Blake, Renee, Jackson, Raina and Martin, Nomi 1991. Rappin on the copula coffin: theoretical and methodological issues in the analysis of copula variation in African-American vernacular English. Language Variation and Change, 3, 103–32.Google Scholar
Rickford, John and McNair-Knox, Faye 1994. Addressee-and topic-influenced style shift: a quantitative sociolinguistic study. In Biber, Douglas and Finegan, Edward (eds), Sociolinguistic Perspectives on Register, 235–76. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie 1994. Acquisition of Variable Rules: (-t,d) Deletion and (ing) Production in Preschool Children. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie 1997. Acquisition of variable rules: a study of (-t,d) deletion in preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 24, 351–72.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie 2002. Child language variation. In Chambers, Jack, Schilling-Estes, Natalie and Trudgill, Peter (eds), Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 333–48. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie 2005. Acquisition of Sociolinguistic variation. In Ball, Martin (ed.), Clinical Sociolinguistics 153164. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie and Labov, William 1995. Learning to talk Philadelphian: acquisition of short a by preschool children. Language Variation and Change 7, 101–12.Google Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne 1975. Linguistic Variability in the Speech of Some Edinburgh Schoolchildren. Unpublished MLitt thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne 1978. Postvocalic /r/ in Scottish English: sound change in progress? In Trudgill, Peter (ed.), Sociolinguistic Patterns in British English, 144–58. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne 1979. The language of Edinburgh schoolchildren: the acquisition of sociolinguistic competence. Scottish Literary Journal, 9, 5561.Google Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne 1984. The Language of Children and Adolescents. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rondal, Jean A. 1984. Linguistic and prelinguistic development in moderate and severe mental retardation. In Dobbing, John, Clarke, A. D. B., Corbett, J. A., Hogg, J. and Robinson, R. O. (eds), Scientific Studies in Mental Retardation, 323–45. London: Royal Society of Medicine and Macmillan.Google Scholar
Rupp, Laura 2005. Constraints on nonstandard -s in expletive there sentences: a generative–variationist perspective. English Language and Linguistics, 9, 255–88.Google Scholar
Ryfa, Joanna 2013. Englishes of London and the Southeast of England. In Hopkins, Tometro, McKenny, John and Decker, Kendall (eds), World Englishes. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline, Brown, Robert and Salerno, Raffaela A. 1976. Adult speech to children. In von Raffler-Engel, Walburga and Lebrun, Yvan (eds), Baby Talk and Infant Speech, 240–5. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Savić, Svenka 1975. Aspects of adult-child communication: the problem of question acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 2 (2), 251–60.Google Scholar
Schleef, Erik 2013. Glottal replacement of /t/ in two British capitals: effects of word frequency and morphological compositionality. Language Variation and Change, 25, 201–23.Google Scholar
Schleef, Erik, Meyerhoff, Miriam and Clark, Lynn 2011. Teenagers’ acquisition of variation: a comparison of locally-born and migrant teens’ realisation of English (ing) in Edinburgh and London. English World-Wide, 32, 206–36.Google Scholar
Schilling-Estes, Natalie 2007. Sociolinguistic fieldwork. In Bayley, Robert and Lucas, Ceil (eds), Sociolinguistic Variation: Theories, Methods, and Applications, 165–89. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schmitt, Cristina and Miller, Karen 2007. Making discourse-dependent decisions: the case of the copulas ser and estar in Spanish. Lingua, 117 (11), 1907–29.Google Scholar
Schütze, Carson T. 1999. English expletive constructions are not infected. Linguistic Inquiry, 30 (3), 467–84.Google Scholar
Schütze, Carson T. and Wexler, Kenneth 1996. Subject case licensing and English root infinitives. In Stringfellow, y, Cahana-Amitay, Dalia, Hughes, Elizabeth and Zukowski, rea (eds), Proceedings of the Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Vol. II, 670–81. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Shriberg, Lawrence D. and Kwlatkowski, Joan 1980. Natural Process Analysis (NPA). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1985/1992. Cross-Linguistic Study of Language Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer 2000. You Ø na hear o’ that kind o’ things: negative do in Buckie Scots. English World-Wide, 21 (2), 231–59.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer 2001. Negative concord in the Old and New World: evidence from Scotland. Language Variation and Change 13 (2), 109–34.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer 2004. Accounting for vernacular features in a Scottish dialect: relic, innovation, analogy and drift. In Kay, Christian, Horobin, Simon and Smith, Jeremy J. (eds), New Perspectives on English Historical Linguistics: Selected Papers from 12 ICEHL, Glasgow, 21–26 August 2002. Amsterdam Studies in the Theory and History of Linguistic Science, 1, 177–93. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer 2013–2016. One Speaker, Two Dialects: Bidialectalism across the Generations in a Scottish Community. Economic and Social Research Council.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer and Durham, Mercedes 2011. A tipping point in dialect obsolescence? Change across the generations in Lerwick, Shetland 1. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 15 (2), 197225.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer and Holmes-Elliott, Sophie 2017. The unstoppable glottal: tracking rapid change in an iconic British variable., English Language and Linguistics, 323355.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, Durham, Mercedes and Fortune, Liane 2007. ‘Mam, ma troosers is fa’in doon!’: community, caregiver and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect. Language Variation and Change, 19 (1), 6399.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, Durham, Mercedes and Fortune, Liane 2009. Universal and dialect-specific pathways of acquisition: caregivers, children, and t/d deletion. Language Variation and Change, 21 (1), 6995.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, Durham, Mercedes and Richards, Hazel 2013. The social and linguistic in the acquisition of sociolinguistic variation. Linguistics, 51 (2), 258324.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine E. 1972. Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development 43, 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine E. 1995. Issues in the study of input: fine tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In Fletcher, Paul and MacWhinney, Brian (eds), The Handbook of Child Language, 180–93. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine E. and Ferguson, Charles A. (eds) 1977. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snyder, William 2007. Child Language: The Parametric Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Song, Jae Yung, Shattuck-Hufnagel, Stefanie and Demuth, Katherine 2015. Development of phonetic variants (allophones) in 2-year-olds learning American English: a study of alveolar stop /t, d/ codas. Journal of Phonetics 52, 152–69.Google Scholar
Sorace, Antonella 2000. Differential effects of attrition in the L1 syntax of near-native L2 speakers. In Catherine Howell, S., Fish, Sarah A. and Keith-Lucas, Thea (eds), Proceedings of the 24th Boston University Conference on Language Development, 719–25. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Sparks, Randall B. 1984. Here’s a few more facts. Linguistic Inquiry 15, 179–83.Google Scholar
Squires, Lauren 2011. Sociolinguistic Priming and the Perception of Agreement Variation: Testing Predictions of Exemplar-theoretic Grammar. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, Carol 1987. Phonological skills of 2-year-olds. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 18, 323–9.Google Scholar
Straw, Michelle and Patrick, Peter L. 2007. Dialect acquisition of glottal variation in /t/: Barbadians in Ipswich. Language Sciences 29 (2–3), 385407.Google Scholar
Stuart-Smith, Jane 1999. Glottals past and present: a study of T-glottalling in Glaswegian. Leeds Studies in English 30, 181204.Google Scholar
Stuart-Smith, Jane 2003. The phonology of Modern Urban Scots. In John Corbett, J. McClure, Derrick and Stuart-Smith, Jane (eds), The Edinburgh Companion to Scots, 110–37. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Stuart-Smith, Jane, Timmins, Claire and Tweedie, Fiona 2006. Conservation and innovation in a traditional dialect: L-vocalization in Glaswegian. English World-Wide, 27 (1), 7187.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. 1998. Was/were variation across the generations: view from the city of York. Language Variation and Change 10, 153–92.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. 2004. Someth[in]’s go[ing] on! Variable ing at ground zero. In Gunnarsson, Britt L., Bergström, Lena, Eklund, Gerd, Staffan Fidell, Lise H. Hansen, Angela Karstadt, Nordberg, Bengt, Sundergren, Eva and Thelander, Mats (eds), Language Variation in Europe: Papers from the Second International Conference on Language Variation in Europe, ICLA VE 2, 390403. Uppsala: Uppsala Universitet.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. 2011. Variationist Sociolinguistics: Change, Observation, Interpretation. Malden/Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. and Baayen, R. Harald 2012. Models, forests, and trees of York English: was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and Change 24 (2), 135–78.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. and Hudson, Rachel 1999. Be like et al. beyond America: the quotative system in British and Canadian youth. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 3 (2), 147–72.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. and Molfenter, Sonja 2007. How’d you get that accent? Acquiring a second dialect of the same language. Language in Society 36, 649–75.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. and Smith, Jennifer 2000. Old was; new ecology: viewing English through the sociolinguistic filter. In Poplack, S. (ed.), The English History of African American English, 147–71. Oxford and Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. and Temple, Rosalind A. M. 2005. New perspectives on an ol’ variable: (-t,d) in British English. Language Variation and Change, 17, 281302.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A., Smith, Jennifer and Lawrence, Helen 2005. No taming the vernacular! Insights from the relatives in northern Britain. Language Variation and Change 17, 75112.Google Scholar
Tannen, Deborah. 1990. You Just Don’t Understand: Women and Men in Conversation. New York, NY: Morrow.Google Scholar
Terry, J. Michael, Hendrick, Randall, Evangelou, Evangelos and Smith, Richard L. 2010. Variable dialect switching among African American children: inferences about working memory. Lingua, 120 (10), 2463–75.Google Scholar
Tollfree, Laura 1999. South East London English: discrete versus continuous modelling of consonantal reduction. In Foulkes, Paul and Docherty, Gerard J. (eds), Urban Voices: Accent Studies in the British Isles, 163–84. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. 1972. Sex, Covert Prestige and Linguistic Change in the Urban British English of Norwich. Language in Society 1(2), 179195.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter 1974. The Social Differentiation of English in Norwich. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter 1986. Dialects in Contact. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter 1999. Norwich: endogenous and exogenous change. In Foulkes, Paul and Docherty, Gerard J. (eds), Urban Voices: Accent Studies in the British Isles, 124–40. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. 2002. Sociolinguistic Variation and Change. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
University of Portsmouth and others 2009–17. Vision of Britain Through Time. GB Historical GIS/Buckie Burgh through time/Population Statistics/Total Population, http://www.visionofbritain.org.uk/unit/10358255/cube/TOT_POP [accessed 24 March 2016].Google Scholar
Vainikka, Anne. 1993. Case in the Development of English syntax. Language Acquisition 3(3), 257325.Google Scholar
Van Hofwegen, Janneke and Stob, Reuben 2011. A longitudinal analysis of the relationship between reading and AAE vernacularity. Paper presented at New Ways of Analyzing Variation (NWAV) 40, October 27–30, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Van Hofwegen, Janneke and Wolfram, Walt 2010. Coming of age in African American English: a longitudinal study. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 1, 427–55.Google Scholar
Visser, Fredericus T. 1963–73. An Historical Syntax of the English Language. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Walker, James A. 2007. ‘There’s bears back there’: plural existentials and vernacular universals in (Quebec) English. English World-Wide, 28, 147–66.Google Scholar
Washington, Julie A. and Craig, Holly K. (2002). Morphosyntactic forms of African American English used by young children and their caregivers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 209–31.Google Scholar
Watt, Dominic 2015. Dangerous and lazy, or God’s own consonant: should we make the glottal stop? Babel 10, 1014.Google Scholar
Weinreich, Uriel, Labov, William and Herzog, Marvin 1968. Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In Lehmann, Winfred P. and Malkiel, Yakov (eds), Directions for Historical Linguistics, 95198. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Weldon, Tracey 1995. Variability in negation in African American vernacular English. English Language Variation and Change, 6, 359–97.Google Scholar
Weldon, Tracey 2003. Copula variability in Gullah. Language Variation and Change, 15 (1), 3772.Google Scholar
Wells, Gordon 1981. Learning Through Interaction: The Study of Language Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wells, John C. 1965. The phonological status of syllabic consonants in English RP. Phonetica, 13, 110–13.Google Scholar
Wells, John C. 1982. Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Westergaard, Marit 2009. Usage-based vs rule-based learning: the acquisition of word order in wh- questions on English and Norwegian. Journal of Child Language, 36, 1023–51.Google Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth 1994. Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations. In Lightfoot, D. and Hornstein, N (eds), Verb Movement, 305–50. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth 1998. Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: a new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 2379.Google Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth 2000. The development of clitics in SLI and normal development: evidence for the unique checking constraint. In Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L. and Wexler, K. (eds), Syntax, Morphology, and Phonology in Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wolfram, Walt 1969. A Sociolinguistic Description of Detroit Negro Speech. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Yang, Charles 2002. Knowledge and Learning in Natural Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Youssef, Valerie 1991. Variation as a feature of language acquisition in the Trinidad context. Language Variation and Change 3, 75101.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Jennifer Smith, University of Glasgow, Mercedes Durham, Cardiff University
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Variation in Children's Language
  • Online publication: 06 May 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316779248.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Jennifer Smith, University of Glasgow, Mercedes Durham, Cardiff University
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Variation in Children's Language
  • Online publication: 06 May 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316779248.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Jennifer Smith, University of Glasgow, Mercedes Durham, Cardiff University
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Variation in Children's Language
  • Online publication: 06 May 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316779248.010
Available formats
×