Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-02T01:28:11.050Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Educating for “intelligent environmental action” in an age of global warming

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2009

Tina Grotzer
Affiliation:
Harvard University
Rebecca Lincoln
Affiliation:
Harvard University
Susanne C. Moser
Affiliation:
National Center for Atmospheric Research, Boulder
Lisa Dilling
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder
Get access

Summary

If you want to understand nature, you must be conversant with the language in which nature speaks to us.

Richard Feynman

What does it mean to be “conversant with the language in which nature speaks to us,” and how do we help others develop this capacity? From a pedagogical perspective, if we want to address climate change and help people become a part of the solution rather than the problem, we must answer this question. One of the authors watched as her two-year-old son took the hand of another little boy on the playground and brought him over to the fence to “see the pretty sunset.” His sense of wonder and enthusiasm for sharing it inspires hope for the future, and yet there is strong evidence that people of all ages understand little of the language or patterns of nature. Too often, as children grow up, they lose their appreciation for and sense of connection to the natural world. This is, in itself, a deep loss. But even if we retain an appreciation for the beauty of nature, few of us ever develop an understanding of the inherent complexities and dynamics of our environment. To solve environmental problems, an intuitive appreciation for nature is certainly necessary, but it is by no means sufficient. How do we learn the patterns of nature? How do we encourage the development of “environmental intelligence” and, more importantly, “intelligent environmental action”?

Type
Chapter
Information
Creating a Climate for Change
Communicating Climate Change and Facilitating Social Change
, pp. 266 - 280
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., et al. (1994). What do people know about global climate change? 1. Mental models. Risk Analysis, 14, 6, 959–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinkman, F. and Boschhuizen, R. (1989). Pre-instructional ideas in biology: A survey in relation with different research methods on concepts of health and energy. In Research and Developments in Teacher Education in the Netherlands, eds. Voorbach, M. T. and Prick, L. G. M.London: Taylor and Francis, pp. 75–90.
Clark, C. F., Kotchen, M. J., and Moore, M. R. (2003). Internal and external influences on pro-environmental behavior: Participation in a green electricity program. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 3, 237–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, R. (1996). Some psychological aspects of reduced consumption behavior: The role of intrinsic satisfaction and competence motivation. Environment and Behavior, 28, 3, 358–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorner, D. (1989). The Logic of Failure. New York: Metropolitan Books.Google Scholar
Driver, R., Guesne, E., and Tiberghien, A. (eds.) (1985). Children's Ideas in Science. Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Driver, R., Leach, J., Scott, P., et al. (1994). Children's ideas about ecology 3: Ideas found in children aged 5–16 about the interdependency of organisms. International Journal of Science Education, 16, 985–97.Google Scholar
Feltovich, P. J., Spiro, R. J., and Coulson, R. L. (1993). Learning, teaching, and testing for complex conceptual understanding. In Test Theory for a New Generation of Tests, eds. Frederiksen, N. and Bejar, I.Hillsdale, NJ: LEA, pp. 181–217.Google Scholar
Feynman, R. (1967). The Character of Physical Law: Messenger Lectures, 1964. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frahm, A., Glavin, D., Gensler, G., et al. (1995). Changing Behavior: Insights and Applications. Behavior Change Project – Final Report. Seattle, WA: King County Water Pollution Control Division.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2000). The Tipping Point: How Little Things Can Make a Big Difference. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.Google Scholar
Glantz, M. H., et al. (1999). Creeping Environmental Problems and Sustainable Development in the Aral Sea Basin. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grotzer, T. A. (1993). Children's Understanding of Complex Causal Relationships in Natural Systems. Unpublished Ed. D. dissertation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Grotzer, T. A. (2002). Causal Patterns in Ecosystems. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Project Zero.Google Scholar
Grotzer, T. A. (2003). Learning to understand the forms of causality implicit in scientific explanations. Studies in Science Education, 39, 1–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grotzer, T. A. (2004). Putting everyday science within reach. Principal Leadership, October, 16–21.Google Scholar
Grotzer, T. A. and Basca, B. B. (2003). How does grasping the underlying causal structures of ecosystems impact students' understanding?Journal of Biological Education, 38, 1, 16–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grotzer, T. A. and Bell, B. (1999). Negotiating the funnel: Guiding students toward understanding elusive generative concepts. In The Project Zero Classroom: Views on Understanding, eds. Hetland, L. and Veenema, S.Fellows and Trustees of Harvard College, pp. 59–75.Google Scholar
Grotzer, T. A. and Sudbury, M. (2000). Moving beyond underlying linear causal models of electrical circuits. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), New Orleans, LA, April.
Kollmuss, A. and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior?Environmental Education Review, 8, 3, 239–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koslowski, B. (1976). Learning about an Instance of Causation. Unpublished manuscript. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.
Leach, J., Driver, R., Scott, P., et al. (1992). Progression in Conceptual Understanding of Ecological Concepts by Pupils Aged 5–16. Leeds, UK: University of Leeds, Centre for Studies in Science and Mathematics Education.Google Scholar
Lesser, H. (1977). The growth of perceived causality in children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 130, 142–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenzie-Mohr, D. (2000). Fostering sustainable behavior through community-based social marketing. American Psychologist, 55, 5, 531–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendelson, R. and Shultz, T. R. (1976). Covariation and temporal contiguity as principles of causal inference in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22, 408–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metz, K. E. (1995). Reassessment of developmental constraints on children's science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 65, 2, 93–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metz, K. E. (1997). On the complex relation between cognitive developmental research and children's science curricula. Review of Educational Research, 67, 1, 151–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Research Council Committee on Abrupt Climate Change Ocean Studies Board, Polar Research Board, Board on Atmospheric Sciences and Climate, Division on Earth and Life Studies (2002). Abrupt Climate Change: Inevitable Surprises. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Penner, D. (2000). Explaining systems: Investigating middle school students' understanding of emergent phenomena. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 8, 784–806.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, D. N. and Grotzer, T. A. (2000). Models and moves: Focusing on dimensions of complex causality to achieve deeper scientific understanding. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA), New Orleans, LA, April.
Perkins, D. N., Tishman, S.Donis, K., et al. (2000). Intelligence in the wild: A dispositional view of intellectual traits. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 3, 269–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, D., Jay, E., and Tishman, S. (1993). Beyond abilities: A dispositional theory of thinking. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 1, 1–21.Google Scholar
Resnick, M. (1994). Turtles, Termites, and Traffic Jams: Explorations in Massively Parallel Microworlds. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Resnick, M. (1996). Beyond the centralized mindset. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 5, 1, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resnick, M. (2003). Thinking like a tree (and other forms of ecological thinking). International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 8, 43–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. and Liebert, R. (1974). Effects of contiguity, regularity, and age on children's causal inferences. Developmental Psychology, 10, 4, 574–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterman, J. D. and Sweeney, L. B. (2002) Cloudy skies: Assessing public understanding of global warming. System Dynamics Review, 18, 2, 101–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomashow, M. (1995). Ecological Identity: Becoming a Reflective Environmentalist. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thomashow, M. (2002). Bringing the Biosphere Home. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
White, B. (1993). Intermediate causal models: A missing link for successful science education. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 1, 1–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilensky, U. (1998). Gaslab – an extensible modeling toolkit for connecting micro- and macro-properties of gases. In Computer Modeling in Science and Mathematics Education, eds. Roberts, N.., and Hunter, B.Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Wilensky, U. and Resnick, M. (1999). Thinking in levels: A dynamic systems approach to making sense of the world. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 8, 1, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, O. (2003). A digital infrastructure for hands-on modeling, simulation and learning of complex causal processes. Unpublished paper, Cambridge, MA: Media Lab, Lifelong Kindergarten Project, MIT.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×