Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:09:59.961Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

PROSODY FACILITATES MEMORY RECALL IN L1 BUT NOT IN L2 IN HIGHLY PROFICIENT LISTENERS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2019

Elaine Schmidt*
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Ana Pérez
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Luca Cilibrasi
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge and Charles University, Prague
Ianthi Tsimpli
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elaine Schmidt, RFB, Sidgwick Avenue, Cambridge, CB3 9DA, United Kingdom. E-mail: aeis2@cam.ac.uk

Abstract

Prosody is crucial for language comprehension because it highlights underlying structures. This study explores whether prosody facilitates memory recall to the same extent in L1 and L2, and whether memory recall is poorer in L2 or whether language-specific differences can mitigate L2 processing difficulties. Nineteen Greek learners of English, and a monolingual English baseline, repeated three-digit chunks with and without prosodic cues in L1 and L2. Prosody was a major facilitator of memory recall only in L1 despite the high proficiency of learners. This indicates that L2 mastery of prosody perception is hard to attain, mirroring production studies. However, when prosodic boundary cues were absent, memory recall in L2 was comparable to L1. This demonstrates that language-specific differences can attenuate more general processing difficulties in L2. This study is the first to demonstrate differences in prosodic processing in L1 and L2 resulting in poorer memory recall in L2.

Type
Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Altenberg, E. P. (2005). The perception of word boundaries in a second language. Second Language Research, 21, 325358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arvaniti, A., & Baltazani, M. (2000). GREEK ToBI: A system for the annotation of Greek speech corpora. Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2000) (pp. 555562). Athens: European Language Resources Association.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A., & Baltazani, M. (2005). Intonational analysis and prosodic annotation of Greek spoken corpora. Prosodic Typology: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing, 84, 117169.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 25, 556559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2013). lme4: linear-mixed effects models using S4 classes . Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.Google Scholar
Berkovits, R. (1993). Progressive utterance-final lengthening in syllables with final fricatives. Language and Speech, 36, 8998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broselow, E., & Finer, D. (1991). Parameter setting in second language phonology and syntax. Interlanguage Studies Bulletin (Utrecht), 7, 3559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C. A. (1998). The role of the L1 grammar in the L2 acquisition of segmental structure. Second Language Research, 14, 136193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrd, D. (2000). Articulatory vowel lengthening and coordination at phrasal junctures. Phonetica, 57, 316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, A. (2003). Language dependence in continuation intonation. In M. Sole, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 10691072). Barcelona: International Phonetic Association.Google Scholar
Chincotta, D., & Hoosain, R. (1995). Reading rate, articulatory suppression and bilingual digit span. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 201211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chincotta, D., & Underwood, G. (1996). Mother tongue, language of schooling and bilingual digit span. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 193208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chincotta, D., & Underwood, G. (1997). Digit span and articulatory suppression: A cross-linguistic comparison. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 8996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Dahan, D., & Van Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature review. Language and Speech, 40, 141201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Rosario-Martínez, H. (2012). Phia: Post-Hoc Interaction Analysis [R package]. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/phia/phia.pdf.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Fletcher, J. (1991). The articulatory kinematics of final lengthening. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 369382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual word‐length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 4351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, A., Miles, J., & Field, Z. (2012). Discovering statistics using R . London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23, 8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grabe, E. (1997). Comparative intonational phonology: English and German. In A. Botinis (Ed.), Intonation: Theory, Models and Applications (pp. 157160). Athens: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA).Google Scholar
Grabe, E. (1998). Pitch accent realization in English and German. Journal of Phonetics, 26, 129143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grabe, E., & Post, B. (2002). Intonational variation in the British Isles. In B. Bel & I. Marlien (Eds.), Proceedings of the Speech Prosody Conference 2002 (pp. 343346). Aix-en-Provence: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA).Google Scholar
Grabe, E., Post, B., Nolan, F., & Farrar, K. (2000). Pitch accent realization in four varieties of British English. Journal of Phonetics, 28, 161185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (2002). Intonation and interpretation: Phonetics and phonology. In B. Bel & I. Marlien (Eds.), Proceedings of the Speech Prosody Conference 2002 (pp. 4757). Aix-en-Provence: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA).Google Scholar
Huang, B. H., & Jun, S. A. (2011). The effect of age on the acquisition of second language prosody. Language and Speech, 54, 387414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jun, S. A., & Oh, M. (2000). Acquisition of second language intonation. Interspeech 2000 (pp. 7376). Beijing: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA).Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Speer, S. R. (1999). Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 153194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. (2017). lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82. doi: 10.18627/jss.v082.i13.Google Scholar
Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Sepanski, S., & Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: A web-based interface for bilingual research. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 202210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathy, F., & Feldman, J. (2012). What’s magic about magic numbers? Chunking and data compression in short-term memory. Cognition, 122, 346362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (2014). Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. London: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Ayres, T. J. (1986). Digit span, reading rate, and linguistic relativity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 739751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., & Ramus, F. (2003). Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants. Speech Communication, 41, 233243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, M. C., & Ellis, N. C. (2000). Cantonese speakers’ memory for English sentences with prosodic cues. The Modern Language Journal, 84, 372389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez, A., Joseph, H. S., Bajo, T., & Nation, K. (2016). Evaluation and revision of inferential comprehension in narrative texts: An eye movement study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 549566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, P. J., Ostendorf, M., Shattuck‐Hufnagel, S., & Fong, C. (1991). The use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90, 29562970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietveld, T., Gussenhoven, C., Wichmann, A., & Grabe, E. (1999). The communicative effects of rising and falling pitch accents in British English and Dutch. Dialogue and Prosody: ESCA Tutorial and Research Workshop (ITRW) (pp. 111116). Veldhoven: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA).Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (1996). Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 606621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Turk, A. (1998). The domain of phrase-final lengthening in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 103, 12351236.Google Scholar
Shipstead, Z., Lindsey, D. R., Marshall, R. L., & Engle, R. W. (2014). The mechanisms of working memory capacity: Primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 116141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigler, J. W., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a temporally limited store. Cognition, 23, 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. S., & Engle, R. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallduví, E., & Engdahl, E. (1996). The linguistic realization of information packaging. Linguistics, 34, 459520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wightman, C. W., Shattuck‐Hufnagel, S., Ostendorf, M., & Price, P. J. (1992). Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91, 17071717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed