Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T16:43:41.027Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 1 - Emotion Communication and Person Perception

from Part I - Basic Processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2023

Ursula Hess
Affiliation:
Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
Reginald B. Adams, Jr.
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Robert E. Kleck
Affiliation:
Dartmouth College, New Hampshire

Summary

An important part of human interaction is the communication of emotions. Emotions can be expressed nonverbally, via the face, the voice, touch and even smell, but also, albeit more rarely verbally, by stating how one feels. Emotion expressions signal not only internal states, but also provide information about the context of the event that elicited the emotion. Facets of the context influence the perception of emotions, and in turn emotions impact person perception. This chapter provides a general overview of the processes underlying emotion perception and person perception and how these processes may be affected as a function of aging.

Type
Chapter
Information
Emotion Communication by the Aging Face and Body
A Multidisciplinary View
, pp. 3 - 24
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

An important part of human interaction is the communication of emotions. In fact, most human interactions involve some level of emotional exchange – be it only a friendly smile in greeting. Emotions can be expressed nonverbally, via the face, the voice, touch, and even smell, but also, albeit more rarely, verbally, by stating how one feels. Yet, by and large, the literature on emotion communication is heavily biased toward the study of facial expressions. In this chapter, we will therefore emphasize research on facial expressions of emotions. However, it should be noted that many of the processes discussed here apply equally to emotion communication via other channels. In real-life interactions, information from multiple channels is available and can be used for sense making. In what follows, we will first discuss what emotion expressions signal to the observer. We then discuss context influences and moderating factors. A last section will focus on person perception and the influence of emotion expressions on trait attributions.

What Do Emotion Expressions Express?

The question of what emotion expressions actually express has been subject to lively discussions in the field. The scientific study of emotion communication is usually traced to Darwin’s seminal work “On the expressions of the emotions in man and animal” (Reference Darwin1965). Darwin’s basic message was that emotion expressions are evolved and (at least at some point in the past) adaptive. He described animal and human emotionally expressive behavior in great detail in order to support this point. Yet, this view has not been unchallenged (see also Reference Hess, Lefebvre and CohenHess, 2017; Reference Lindquist, Siegel, Quigley and BarrettLindquist et al., 2013).

Although there is relative agreement that emotion expressions have a communicative function, there is decided disagreement on what exactly they communicate and whether this communication reflects the internal state of the organism (Reference DarwinDarwin, 1965; Reference FridlundFridlund, 1994; Reference Lindquist, Siegel, Quigley and BarrettLindquist et al., 2013). Most prominently, Fridlund’s behavioral ecology theory (Reference FridlundFridlund, 1994) contradicted Darwin’s assumption of the usefulness of the honest communication of emotional states. According to Fridlund, for emotion expressions to be truly useful as a communicative signal they should be linked to the organism’s social motives rather than to the underlying emotional state of the communicator. From this perspective, emotion expressions should be considered as indicative of social motives and not of emotions per se. More recently, Reference Barrett, Mesquita and GendronBarrett, Mesquita, and Gendron (2011) proposed that facial expressions do not in fact provide any specific information by themselves. Because emotion expressions are ambiguous in their signal value, it is proposed that their meaning is primarily constructed in light of the context of the interaction. This latter view is actually not much different from other approaches that highlight the importance of context and real-world knowledge for emotion communication (Reference Adams, Ambady, Nakayama and ShimojoAdams et al., 2011; Reference Hess, Hareli, Abell and SmithHess & Hareli, 2016). Yet, the relative emphasis of these two general approaches differs with one assuming that emotion expressions do have meaning per se while the other focuses more strongly on the influence of contextual factors.

Notably, these discussions focus on the “true” meaning of the expressions, that is, on the question of whether expressions reflect some ground truth about the expresser. In stark contrast to this perspective stands the use that observers make of emotion expressions. Specifically, as is amply demonstrated by the use of facial expressions in the arts, for example films and literature, perceivers understand emotional facial expressions to express underlying emotional states and they react as a function of this understanding (see Reference Niedenthal and BrauerNiedenthal & Brauer, 2012).

That is, perceivers treat emotion expressions as if they were reliable indicators of emotions and act in accordance with their perceptions. They use this information about the sender’s emotional state to infer not only how the expresser feels but also what the person might do next, that is, what are their action tendencies (e.g., Reference FrijdaFrijda, 1987; Reference Frijda, Kuipers and ter ShureFrijda, Kuipers, & ter Shure, 1989); what might be a likely cause of the emotion as implied by appraisal patterns associated with specific emotions (e.g., Reference Fontaine, Scherer and SorianoFontaine, Scherer, & Soriano, 2013; Reference RosemanRoseman, 1991; Reference Roseman, Spindel and JoseRoseman, Spindel, & Jose, 1990); and even what the expresser might want the observer to do, that is, the appeal associated with the expression (Reference ScarantinoScarantino, 2019; Reference Scarantino, Hareli and HessScarantino et al., 2022). Next to these functions of emotion expressions, which relate to the immediate situational context, emotion expressions also impact on person perception. Thus, emotion expressions can signal the expresser’s values and motivations (Reference Hess and HareliHess & Hareli, 2019) and more generally their character (Reference Hareli and HessHareli & Hess, 2010).

Emotion Perception and Social Context

There are two principal strategies for decoding emotion displays (Reference Kirouac, Hess, Philippot, Feldman and CoatsKirouac & Hess, 1999). First, the sender’s expressions can be used to draw inferences regarding his or her presumed emotional state using a pattern-matching approach (Reference Buck and BuckBuck, 1984). However, a second strategy depends upon the knowledge that the perceiver possesses regarding both the sender and the social situation in which the interaction is taking place. This information permits the perceiver to take the perspective of the encoder and helps him or her to correctly infer the emotional state that the sender is most likely experiencing. Recent research suggests that these two processes actually draw on different brain areas and result in different types of emotion judgments (Reference Antypa, Kafetsios and SimosAntypa et al., 2022; Reference Hess and KafetsiosHess & Kafetsios, 2022).

The Active Observer

The traditional view of emotion communication presumes that the decoder “reads out” the emotional state of the interaction partner based on the pattern-matching process just described. In this view the decoder remains passive, that is, no active sense making is involved. It should be noted, however, that in everyday life, emotion expressions are often weak, elusive, or blended, resulting in a signal that is often ambiguous (Reference Motley and CamdenMotley & Camden, 1988). This ambiguity suggests that significant interpretive work is required on the part of a perceiver.

The social context of the interaction is a primary source of information that can be used to disambiguate such expressions. First, knowing what elicits the emotion, observers can use this information in conjunction with their knowledge about what sort of event will likely elicit a given emotion to guide their perception, particularly since people are aware of the typical relations between perceived features of a situation and the resulting emotions (Reference ParkinsonParkinson, 1999, Reference Parkinson, Scherer, Schorr and Johnstone2001; Reference Roseman and EvdokasRoseman & Evdokas, 2004). Firsthand knowledge about an interaction partner is another useful source of information. Knowing, for example, that a person is prone to anger, may bias the observer’s judgment in that direction. Cultural rules and norms provide yet another guide. In cultures in which the expression of a given emotion is proscribed via display rules (Reference Ekman and ColeEkman, 1972) or via decoding rules (Reference Buck and BuckBuck, 1984), the detection of this emotion tends to be impaired.

It should be noted that in many real-life situations we may not have access to the sources of information listed here. In such cases, stereotype knowledge about the interaction partner can be a source of relevant decoding information. That is, the social group to which an expresser belongs can provide stereotype information about the expresser’s likely emotions. Knowing a person’s gender or age – information that can be gleaned from the face, but also from sources such as voice (Reference Lass, Hughes, Bowyer, Waters and BourneLass et al., 1976; Reference LinvilleLinville, 1996) or gait (e.g., Reference Montepare and ZebrowitzMontepare & Zebrowitz, 1993) – provides access to stereotypes about the expresser’s emotionality, which can then influence the interpretation of expressions.

In this vein, in a seminal study, Reference Hugenberg and BodenhausenHugenberg and Bodenhausen (2003) showed participants otherwise identical avatars which were either black or white, with an expression that changed from neutral to angry. The participants were faster to detect anger in the black (male) avatar. Importantly, the speed of detection correlated with the bias shown in a race IAT (Implicit Association Test; Reference McGheeGreenwald et al., 1998). That is, the stereotype that links black males with threat influenced the perception of the expression.

Finally, the perceivers’ own goals and needs, and even their own emotional state (see Reference Showers and CantorShowers & Cantor, 1985) influence emotion perception. Thus, participants who are more motivated to decode the emotions of others – because they are members of an ingroup whose emotions are of more interest (Reference Thibault, Bourgeois and HessThibault, Bourgeois, & Hess, 2006), or because accuracy is rewarded (Reference Hess, Blaison and DandeneauHess, Blaison, & Dandeneau, 2017) – are more accurate in decoding emotion expressions.

This discussion has important implications for the perception of emotions in elderly individuals. First, age stereotypes can influence emotion perception. A recent study found that participants implicitly associated young adult individuals with positive emotions, specifically happiness and serenity, and old adult individuals with the negative emotions of sadness and anger. Within negative emotions, participants preferentially associated young adult individuals with sadness and old adult individuals with anger (Reference Freudenberg, Albohn, Kleck, Adams and HessFreudenberg et al., 2020).

Not only the specific stereotypes that people may hold about the emotions that the elderly may experience, but also stereotypes regarding the personality of the elderly can bias emotion perception. Thus, individuals who perceive the elderly according to a stereotype that views them as weak and incompetent (Reference Cuddy, Fiske and NelsonCuddy & Fiske, 2002) may tend to attribute sadness, which is associated with lack of coping potential (Reference SchererScherer, 1997) rather than anger, which is associated with high coping potential, to an ambiguous expression by an older person.

The fact that Reference Malatesta, Izard, Culver and NicolichMalatesta and colleagues (1987) found that individuals are better at recognizing expressions by those closer in age than those farther removed in age, opens the possibility that there may be age-related expressive dialects just like there are culture-related nonverbal dialects (Reference Elfenbein and AmbadyElfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Reference Elfenbein, Beaupré, Levesque and HessElfenbein et al., 2007). Such age-related nonverbal dialects would result in subtle differences in expressions between young and old adults which would then make it more difficult for someone from one age group to decode the expressions of a different age cohort.

Finally, the visual salience of an encoder may play a role. Specifically, the elderly are often referred to as an “invisible demographic,” a notion that expands beyond political visibility to the notion that they are also overlooked or disregarded in everyday interactions. This more fleeting attention may then result in reduced decoding accuracy, particularly if the expression is ambiguous.

Moderating Factors for Emotion Expression and Recognition

A number of influences on emotion communication have been studied over the years. Next to the question of cultural influences – as discussed previously – an often-raised issue regards gender differences in nonverbal communication, which overlaps with the question regarding status differences. These two issues will be briefly outlined next. Notably, as stereotypic views of the elderly often include notions of weakness and lack of status, the effects of perceived low status on emotional communication are especially relevant for this group.

Gender and Status

Differences in the expression and recognition of emotion displays have been found with regard to the status and gender of both expresser and decoder. Generally speaking, women are more emotionally expressive than men (Reference FischerFischer, 1993). This is best established for smiling, such that women smile more than men particularly in situations where they experience negative affect. This difference emerges in childhood and gets stronger by the time women reach adulthood (for a review see, Reference Hess, Beaupré, Cheung, Abel and CeiaHess, Beaupré, & Cheung, 2002). By contrast, men are perceived, and perceive themselves, as more likely to express anger when in a negative emotional state. Interestingly, in experimental situations where anger is induced in both men and women this difference disappears (Reference FischerFischer, 1993).

The reasons for these well-established gender differences have been traced to two – nonexclusive – sources: differences in status and differences in social roles. Thus, Reference HenleyHenley (1977, Reference Henley, Kalbfleisch and Cody1995) as well as LaFrance and Hecht (Reference Hecht and LaFranceHecht & LaFrance, 1998; Reference LaFrance and HechtLaFrance & Hecht, 1995) emphasize the importance of the inherent difference in status between men and women, which maintains to this day even in so-called egalitarian cultures (e.g., Reference RidgewayRidgeway, 2011). Henley in particular, bases her argument on the assumption that the human smile is a homologue of the primate silent-bared-teeth display, which typically is used as a sign of submission. From Henley’s perspective human smiles signal submission and hence women, as the lower status gender, tend to smile more. This model may be a bit too simplistic though. On one hand people who smile tend to be rated as dominant (Reference KnutsonKnutson, 1996) and there is only limited evidence linking smiling as such to status and power. In fact, there are many different forms of smiles that serve different social functions, with the submissive smile being just one (Reference Niedenthal, Mermillod, Maringer and HessNiedenthal et al., 2010). In this vein, Reference Brody, Hall, Lewis and HavilandBrody and Hall (2000) propose a more complex model, which includes social norms regarding gender adequate behavior, social expectations, but also a stronger trend toward positive affect experience in women than in men.

As regards anger expressions in men, status seems to be more clearly relevant. Thus, Reference Averill, Hogan, Johnson and BriggsAverill (1997) considers power an “entrance requirement” for anger. The notion being that the anger display of a person who does not have power to back up the threat is less effective and in fact less legitimate. As an example, one may think of the angry temper tantrum of a child versus an angry expression of a member of a biker gang. This view concords with the position of appraisal theories of emotion which consider coping potential – the power to redress a situation – as the key appraisal for anger (Reference Ellsworth, Scherer, Davidson, Goldsmith and SchererEllsworth & Scherer, 2003).

Emotion Expression in Other Channels

As mentioned previously, even though facial displays are the most frequently studied channel for emotion expression, emotions are obviously also expressed through other channels. In what follows, we will give a short overview of research on emotion expression through voice, posture, and gaze.

Voice

Research on emotion expression in the voice has been ongoing since it has become technically feasible to record voices. However, until about ten years ago, the frequency of studies per year in this domain remained low. In fact, just like research on posture and touch, research on voice gained impact with the advent of affective computing and the accompanying interest in automated affective sensing (Reference Schuller, Batliner, Steidl and SeppiSchuller et al., 2011).

There are two main approaches to classifying emotion relevant speech. On one hand, human perceivers can be asked to listen to voice excerpts and infer the emotions expressed. On the other, acoustic features of the emotional voice such as pitch, duration and intensity, or voice quality features can be measured and related to the intended emotion (Reference Juslin, Scherer, Harrigan, Rosenthal and SchererJuslin & Scherer, 2005). Research employing the judgment study paradigm was able to ascertain that so-called basic emotions are well recognized in speech, but early attempts at acoustic analysis were not always as successful (Reference Banse and SchererBanse & Scherer, 1996). However, newer approaches in affective computing using more sophisticated analysis algorithms have started to make inroads in this regard (Reference Schuller, Batliner, Steidl and SeppiSchuller et al., 2011).

Posture

Reference DarwinDarwin’s (1965) analyses of emotion expressions included many descriptions of emotional postures in both humans and animals. However, following Darwin, emotion-specific postures were rarely studied. Reference Ekman and FriesenEkman and Friesen (1974), for example, considered postures only indicative of the intensity of an emotion and not of its quality. Yet, even early studies by Bull and colleagues (e.g., Reference Bull and Gidro-FrankBull & Gidro-Frank, 1950) suggested that some basic emotions can be recognized from postures. In recent years, interest in postures has blossomed again. Work on static body cues suggests that at least the basic emotions can be well recognized from postural cues alone (see Reference Atkinson and VuilleumierAtkinson, 2013, for a review). Other work has shown that basic emotions can also be recognized from gait at levels that are comparable to facial emotion recognition, ranging up to 92 percent correct for sad and fearful expressions (Reference Schneider, Christensen and HäußingerSchneider et al., 2013). In addition, emotions such as pride and others that are not considered as basic (mainly because they are not associated with a prototypical and unique facial expression) seem by contrast to have a universal postural component (Reference Tracy and MatsumotoTracy & Matsumoto, 2008). Further, there is evidence of cross-model mimicry of postures such that individuals who observe emotional postures tend to show congruent facial expressions in response (Reference Magnée, Stekelenburg, Kemner and de GelderMagnée et al., 2007), suggesting that observers react to postural emotion cues in much the same way as to facial emotion cues.

Posture is strongly affected in elderly individuals. For example, older persons have more trouble maintaining an upright posture (Reference Teasdale, Bard and DadouchiTeasdale et al., 1992), which is one signal of dominance. A more “slumped” posture is associated with sadness rather than, for example, anger, and hence may bias emotion perception in this direction.

Gaze

Gaze direction is something not usually thought to be part of the emotional expression itself (Reference Ellsworth and RossEllsworth & Ross, 1975; Reference Fehr, Exline, Siegman and FeldsteinFehr & Exline, 1987). Indeed, the faces employed in nearly all expression decoding studies have used stimuli where the expresser’s gaze is directed at the perceiver. The general argument made concerning the effect of direct gaze is that it plays an important role in the perception of the intensity of the emotion but not in the perception of its quality (Reference Argyle and CookArgyle & Cook, 1976; Reference KleinkeKleinke, 1986; Reference WebbinkWebbink, 1986). An obvious reason this might be the case is that direct gaze signals that the perceiver is the object of whatever emotion is being displayed by the expresser and thus captivates attentional resources (Reference CaryCary, 1978; Reference Ellsworth and RossEllsworth & Ross, 1975; Reference Grumet, Guerrero and DeVitoGrumet, 1999; Reference Macrae, Hood, Milne, Rowe and MasonMacrae et al., 2002). By contrast, research by Adams and his colleagues (Reference Adams, Gordon, Baird, Ambady and KleckAdams et al., 2003; Reference Adams and KleckAdams & Kleck, 2003, Reference Adams and Kleck2005) support the shared signal hypothesis, demonstrating that the gaze direction of the expresser can affect the efficiency with which a given display is processed as well as determine the quality of the emotion that will be perceived in a blended or ambiguous expression. They argue that when different facial cues such as the specific expression and the direction of gaze share the same signal value (e.g., approach or avoidance) the shared signal facilitates overall processing efficiency. Others have reported evidence supporting perceptual integration in the processing of these cues. These studies also demonstrate that when gaze and emotion are not of relatively equal discriminability, direct gaze effects do occur (e.g., Reference Graham and LaBarGraham & LaBar, 2007), and when emotion expression is more ambiguous, the shared signal effects emerge (Reference Graham and LaBarGraham & LaBar, 2012). Thus, gaze direction appears to not only influence emotion perception but does so through the processes of both indirect attention capture and direct perceptual integration.

Nonverbal Behavior in Dyads

Research on nonverbal behavior has long focused either on the factors that influence how the expresser encodes certain traits or states or on the factors that influence how the perceiver decodes these traits or states. Yet, social interaction implies an interplay of encoding and decoding. One of the phenomena that occur in a dyadic context is behavioral synchronization. Early research on speech for example, noted that as an interaction progresses, the interaction partners converge with regard to certain characteristics of speech such as loudness and speed (Reference Giles, Smith, Giles and ClairGiles & Smith, 1979). The person who initially speaks louder and faster becomes softer and slower, and the converse for the other person. This convergence is linked to the amount of rapport between the interaction partners (Reference Giles, Smith, Giles and ClairGiles & Smith, 1979). Other research looked at behavioral synchronization and its effect on both experienced and perceived rapport (Reference Bavelas, Black, Lemery and MullettBavelas et al., 1986; Reference Bernieri, Rosenthal, Feldman and RiméBernieri & Rosenthal, 1991). This research was taken up and made popular by Reference Chartrand and BarghChartrand and Bargh (1999) who coined the term chameleon effect to describe the similarities of nonverbal behaviors such as foot tapping and face touching between two interaction partners. As behavioral synchronization fosters affiliation, it has also been referred to as “social glue” (Reference Lakin, Jefferis, Cheng and ChartrandLakin et al., 2003).

Facial Mimicry

A related but somewhat different phenomenon is facial or emotional mimicry which refers specifically to the imitation of emotional behavior (Reference Hess and FischerHess & Fischer, 2013). Facial mimicry is often considered a form of affective empathy or a “low road” in the empathy process (Reference WalterWalter, 2012).

The Social Regulatory Function of Mimicry

Emotional mimicry seems to serve a social regulatory function in dyads (see Reference Hess and FischerHess & Fischer, 2013) and depends on the relationship between interaction partners and more generally on their goals and intentions. Emotional mimicry has relational implications: Emotionally mimicking others can create social warmth but also social coolness when people do not mimic the other.

Thus, whether the relationship with the other is cooperative or competitive (Reference Lanzetta and EnglisLanzetta & Englis, 1989; Reference Weyers, Mühlberger, Kund, Hess and PauliWeyers et al., 2009), or whether one identifies with the expresser as a member of a specific group (Reference Bourgeois and HessBourgeois & Hess, 2008) are factors demonstrated to moderate mimicry. More generally, a negative attitude toward the expresser tends to inhibit emotional mimicry and increase the interpretation of the emotional signal as hostile (e.g., Reference Hutchings and HaddockHutchings & Haddock, 2008). Interestingly, Reference Likowski, Mühlberger, Seibt, Pauli and WeyersLikowski and colleagues (2008) demonstrated that this is the case even when attitudes are newly formed. They showed participants computer generated faces together with narratives about a specific character. Only expressions by avatars who were presented as “good” in these narratives were consistently mimicked.

In line with affiliation at the individual level, affiliation at the group level also fosters mimicry. Thus, individuals are more likely to mimic the emotional reactions of in-group members than those of out-group members (Reference Bourgeois and HessBourgeois & Hess, 2008; Reference van der Schalk, Fischer and Doosjevan der Schalk et al., 2011).

More recently, there has been an increased interest in top-down processes, such as social judgments, which can influence perception-action coupling, including mimicry (Reference Cracco, Genschow and BaessCracco, Genschow, & Baess, 2022). In particular, social judgments about the appropriateness of an emotion expression influence emotional mimicry such that expressions that are considered to be inappropriate are not mimicked (Reference Kastendieck, Mauersberger, Blaison, Ghalib and HessKastendieck et al., 2020; Reference Mauersberger, Kastendieck, Hetmann, Schöll and HessMauersberger et al., 2022), an effect that is mediated by a desire to distance oneself from someone who does not adhere to emotion norms such as smiling at weddings. This might also be relevant for the mimicry of older individuals as socio-cultural expression norms can change over time, potentially rendering some emotion expressions less appropriate to younger observers. Hess, Chapter 8, this volume, discusses mimicry of older individuals in more detail.

Person Perception/First Impressions

People rapidly and spontaneously make judgments about the personality of others based on appearance cues (see e.g., Reference KennyKenny, 2004; Reference Todorov and UlemanTodorov & Uleman, 2002, Reference Todorov and Uleman2003). One classic study found that varying only one aspect of a photo (e.g., adding glasses) impacts on the perceived personality of the depicted person (Reference ThorntonThornton, 1943). But appearance also sends signals regarding the social group membership of the person, including such aspects as gender, age, and ethnicity, all of which, as we noted earlier, impact on emotion perception.

It should be mentioned that there is some disagreement on how accurate first impressions are. Earlier approaches, such as physiognomy, which attempted to link certain facial features with specific traits (Reference LavaterLavater, 1804) have been thoroughly debunked. Looking at people’s naïve theories and judgments, there is evidence for both accuracy (see Schwartzman and Rule, Chapter 2, this volume) and inaccuracy (see Franklin, Chapter 4, this volume). One reason why such judgments may be accurate was proposed by Reference Berry and BrownlowBerry and Brownslow (1989), who suggest that facial features may in fact shape behavior. They propose, for example, that, with time, babyfaced individuals adopt behaviors which fit their appearance and thus become what their face announces them to be.

Obviously, the decision of whether first impressions are correct or incorrect is often a matter of degree. Correlations between impressions and actual personality traits are generally low to medium in size. Accuracy is often above chance but far from perfect. Whether this counts as accurate may depend on the criterion that is used (for a more complete discussion see, e.g., Reference Biesanz, Wallace, Matthews and CorrBiesanz & Wallace, 2020). As such, first impressions often are somewhat correct but seldom fully correct. Yet, from a social psychological perspective even when being incorrect some of the time, first impressions still provide the observer with some idea of how to approach or respond to unknown others. At the same time it is important to keep in mind that first impressions are hard to correct as this requires both motivation and cognitive resources (Reference Mann and FergusonMann & Ferguson, 2015), see also, Chapter 7, this volume.

One important source of first impressions is facial appearance (Reference ZebrowitzZebrowitz, 1997). A line of research, which has a long history, has focused on physical attractiveness (see also Sutherland and Young, Chapter 7, this volume) and on stereotype and halo effects that associate attractiveness with other desirable traits, sometimes labeled the “what is beautiful is good” stereotype (Reference FelsonFelson, 1979; Reference Reis, Wilson, Monestere and BernsteinReis et al., 1990).

Another line of research has focused on the static aspects of facial and bodily appearance cues that can signal general dispositions and behavioral intentions. For example, a square jaw, high forehead, or heavy eyebrows cross-culturally connote social dominance (Reference Keating, Mazur and SegallKeating, Mazur, & Segall, 1981; Reference Keating, Mazur and SegallKeating, Mazur, Segall, et al., 1981; Reference Senior, Phillips, Barnes and DavidSenior et al., 1999). On the other hand, a rounded face with large eyes, thin eyebrows, and low facial features – a babyface – connotes approachability (e.g., Reference Berry and McArthurBerry & McArthur, 1985).

These behavioral dispositions cued by facial appearance are of central importance for our interactions with others as they allow us to infer the social characteristics of an individual we are about to interact with. In hierarchical primate societies, for example, highly dominant individuals pose a certain threat insofar as they can claim territory or possessions (food, sexual partners, etc.) from lower status group members (Reference Menzel and MenzelMenzel, 1973, Reference Menzel, Schrier and Stollnitz1974). Hence the presence of a perceived dominant other should lead to increased vigilance and a preparedness for withdrawal (Reference Coussi-KorbelCoussi-Korbel, 1994). In contrast, an affiliation motive is associated with nurturing, supportive behaviors and should lead to approach when the other is perceived to be high on this disposition.

Interestingly, the same information that is transmitted by relatively static morphological cues can also be transmitted by movement behaviors, including facial expressions. Of these, anger, happiness, and fear displays have been shown to be associated with perceived dominance and affiliation. Accordingly, drawing the eyebrows together in anger leads to increased attributions of dominance, whereas smiling leads to increased attributions of affiliation (Reference Hess, Blairy and KleckHess, Blairy, & Kleck, 2000; Reference KnutsonKnutson, 1996). At the same time, anger expressions are perceived as threatening (e.g., Reference Aronoff, Woike and HymanAronoff, Woike, & Hyman, 1992), whereas smiles are perceived as warm, friendly, and welcoming (see e.g., Reference Hess, Beaupré, Cheung, Abel and CeiaHess et al., 2002). Similarly, it has been argued that fear expressions elicit affiliative reactions in conspecifics (Reference Bauer and GariépyBauer & Gariépy, 2001; Reference Marsh, Ambady and KleckMarsh, Ambady, & Kleck, 2005). This notion of a perceptual overlap between emotion expressions and certain trait markers, which then influences emotion communication, has been more recently taken up by Zebrowitz (see Reference Zebrowitz, Montepare, Schaller, Simpson and KendrickZebrowitz & Montepare, 2006) as well as Reference Hess, Adams, Kleck, Hess and PhilippotHess, Adams, and Kleck (2007, Reference Hess, Adams, Kleck, Ambady and Skowronski2008, Reference Hess, Adams, Kleck, Demoulin, Leyens and Dovidio2009). Derived from this is the notion that the resemblance of certain facial features to emotion expressions drives the attribution of behavioral intentions to some types of faces (Reference Zebrowitz, Fellous, Mignault and AndreolettiZebrowitz et al., 2003). That appearance cues can have lasting effects on individuals was shown for example by Reference Mueller and MazurMueller and Mazur (1996), who found the perceived dominance of West Point cadets, based on photos alone, was a predictor of later military rank.

Further, emotion expressions can be used to infer a person’s character – by a process called reverse engineering (Reference Hareli and HessHareli & Hess, 2010). Specifically, according to appraisal theories of emotion a relevant change in the internal or external environment is evaluated according to a number of dimensions, such as whether the event is pleasant or unpleasant (pleasantness) or whether the change is in line with the motivational state of the individual or obstructs the individual’s goals. Specific emotions are differentiated by the pattern of appraisals they are the result of. Thus, anger is an emotion that is characterized by appraisals of goal obstruction, high coping potential and a perception of norm violation. By contrast, sadness is characterized by appraisals of goal obstruction, but combined with low coping potential, with norms playing less of a role (Reference Scherer, Scherer and EkmanScherer, 1984). Importantly, the way a person appraises a given situation is specific to the individual and the individual’s current state. Factors such as the personality and skills of the person determine their resources, values, and motivations. These in turn define the outcome of their appraisal of an event.

Notably, people’s naïve emotion theories tend to be largely consistent with appraisal theory (Reference HareliHareli, 2014). Hence, people can – based on their naïve emotion theories that represent the appraisals – reconstruct the appraisals of other people’s emotions (e.g., Reference Hess, Hareli, Russell and Fernandez-DolsHess & Hareli, 2017; Reference Manstead, Fischer, Scherer, Schorr and JohnstoneManstead & Fischer, 2001; Reference RosemanRoseman, 1991; Reference Scherer and GrandjeanScherer & Grandjean, 2008). This in turn provides insights into their goals, values, and motivations and through these into their character (Reference de Melo, Carnevale, Read and Gratchde Melo et al., 2014; Reference Hareli and HessHareli & Hess, 2010). For example, an observer, who sees a person react with anger to an injustice can conclude that the person has values according to which the event in question appears unjust, perceives this injustice as incongruent with their own motivational state (which would be to see justice done) and also feels competent to act accordingly. It is important to note in this context that perceivers see emotions even in actually neutral, non-expressive faces (Reference Adams, Nelson, Soto, Hess and KleckAdams et al., 2012) and these perceptions then entrain corresponding person perceptions.

Person Perception and the Aging Face

First impressions of older individuals can be affected by a number of factors, which are also likely to interact. First, as noted above, age stereotypes associate the elderly with a range of traits ranging from weak to wise (see Hummert, Chapter 6, this volume). Second, the aging face tends to be perceived as less attractive (Reference Korthase and TrenholmeKorthase & Trenholme, 1982), see also Chapter 7, this volume. This effect tends to be stronger for female faces and stronger for younger and middle-aged perceivers than older perceivers (Reference Foos and ClarkFoos & Clark, 2011). As noted earlier, attractiveness has a strong halo effect on other trait attributions – a phenomenon that is often labeled “what is beautiful is good” (Reference Dion, Berscheid and WalsterDion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972). As such, a reduction in perceived attractiveness as a function of aging can be expected to entrain a less positive first impression overall.

Using avatars that had the same underlying facial structure and only differed in skin texture to control for the effect of such morphological features as high cheekbones or face shape, Reference Huppertz and MauersbergerHess and colleagues (2023) asked 387 participants to rate the neutral expressions of old and young avatars. They found that young appearing avatars were rated overall more positively and warm than old appearing avatars. This effect was found for both female and male avatars but was stronger for female avatars and mapped closely onto the effect for attractiveness. Only with regard to perceived maturity was the effect reversed. Interestingly, young female avatars were rated as more mature than young male avatars, an effect that disappeared for the older avatars.

This study also showed that participants saw generally more negative emotions in the neutral face of old appearing avatars. Yet, as noted above, such emotion perceptions also influence trait judgments. For example, ratings of pleasantness and warmth were strongly influenced by the intensity of perceived anger in the neutral faces.

In sum, the wrinkles and folds of older faces can be misperceived as emotion signals (Reference Hess, Adams, Simard, Stevenson and KleckHess et al., 2012) and these erroneously perceived emotions impact on trait ratings. As the above study suggests that this is mainly the case for negative emotions (more happiness was perceived in the faces of young appearing avatars), the effect on person perception tends to be overall more negative. Further, older faces are perceived as less attractive, which also leads to more negative overall trait judgments. It should be noted, however, that stereotypes of the elderly include both positive and negative traits (see Chapter 6, this volume).

Conclusions

Nonverbal behavior plays an important role in the communication of emotions and personality traits. Nonverbal communication is embedded in a social context, which influences how expressions are perceived and interpreted. As such, stereotypes, social rules and norms, as well as the situational context and the social group membership of the expresser, can all influence the perception of emotions. First impressions are also influenced by these same factors. With regard to the perception of the elderly this implies complex interactions at the intersection of age, sex, and status.

References

Adams, R. B. Jr., Ambady, N., Nakayama, K., & Shimojo, S. (2011). Social Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr., Gordon, H. L., Baird, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Effect of gaze on Amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science, 300, 15361537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 644647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., Nelson, A. J., Soto, J. A., Hess, U., & Kleck, R. E. (2012). Emotion in the neutral face: A mechanism for impression formation? Cognition and Emotion, 26, 431441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antypa, D., Kafetsios, K., Simos, P., et al. (2022). Neural correlates of contextualized facial emotion perception and its links to personality. Paper presented at the Conference of the Consortium of European Research on Emotion (CERE), Granada, Spain, June 1011.Google Scholar
Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., & Hyman, L. M. (1992). Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 10501066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, A. P. (2013). Bodily expressions of emotion: Visual cues and neural mechanisms. In Vuilleumier, J. A. P. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience (pp. 198222). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1997). The emotions: An integrative approach. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J. A., & Briggs, S. R. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 513541). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 286290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, D. J., & Gariépy, J. L. (2001). The functions of freezing in the social interactions of juvenile high-and low-aggressive mice. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 463475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In Feldman, R. S. & Rimé, B. (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401432). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berry, D. S., & Brownlow, S. (1989). Were the physiognomists right? Personality correlates of facial babyishness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 266279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. Z. (1985). Some components and consequences of a babyface. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 312323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biesanz, J. C., & Wallace, J. D. (2020). Accuracy in person perception. In Matthews, G. & Corr, P. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 4455). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77, 343352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender, emotion, and expression. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 447460). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (1984). Nonverbal receiving ability. In Buck, R. (Ed.), The communication of emotion (pp. 209242). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bull, N., & Gidro-Frank, L. (1950). Emotions induced and studied in hypnotic subjects: Part II – The findings. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 112, 97120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cary, M. S. (1978). The role of gaze in the initiation of conversation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 41, 269271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coussi-Korbel, S. (1994). Learning to outwit a competitor in mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 164171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cracco, E., Genschow, O., & Baess, P. (2022). Top-down social modulation of perception-action coupling. Acta Psychologica, 222, 103481. DOI : https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103481CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuddy, A. J. C., & Fiske, S. T. (2002). Doddering but dear: Process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In Nelson, T. D. (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons (pp. 326). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. (Originally published, 1872)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 7388. http://10.1037/a0034251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(3), 285290. http://10.1037/h0033731CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971 (pp. 207283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Detecting deception from the body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(3), 288298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203235. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M. G., Levesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7, 131146. http://10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Ross, L. (1975). Intimacy in response to direct gaze. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11(6), 592613. http://10.1016/0022-1031(75)90010-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Goldsmith, H., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of the affective sciences (pp. 572595). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, B. J., & Exline, R. V. (1987). Nonverbal behavior and communication In Siegman, A. W. & Feldstein, S. (Eds.), Social visual interaction: A conceptual and literature review (pp. 225325). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Felson, R. B. (1979). “Are the good beautiful or the beautiful good?” The relationship between children’s perception of ability and perceptions of physical attractiveness. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42(4), 386392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A. H. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype? Feminism & Psychology, 3, 303318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (2013). Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foos, P. W., & Clark, M. C. (2011). Adult Age and gender differences in perceptions of facial attractiveness: Beauty is in the eye of the older beholder. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172(2), 162175. http://10.1080/00221325.2010.526154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freudenberg, M., Albohn, D. N., Kleck, R. E., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Hess, U. (2020). Emotional stereotypes on trial: Implicit emotion associations for young and old adults. Emotion, 20, 12441254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion: Cognitive structure and action readiness. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 115143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Shure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion appraisal and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, H., & Smith, P. (1979). Accommodation theory: Optimal levels of convergence. In Giles, H. & Clair, R. N. S. (Eds.), Language and social psychology (pp. 4565). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Garner interference reveals dependencies between emotional expression and gaze in face perception. Emotion, 7(2), 296313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2012). Neurocognitive mechanisms of gaze-expression interactions in face processing and social attention. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 553566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, McGhee, A. G., D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480.Google Scholar
Grumet, G. W. (1999). Kinesic cues: The body, eyes, and face. In Guerrero, L. K & DeVito, J. A. (Eds.), The nonverbal communication reader: Classic and contemporary readings (pp. 4889). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.Google Scholar
Hareli, S. (2014). Making sense of the social world and influencing it by using a naïve attribution theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 336343. http://10.1177/1754073914534501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128140. http://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802613828CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, M. A., & LaFrance, M. (1998). License or obligation to smile: The effect of power and sex on amount and type of smiling. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(12), 13321342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex and nonverbal communication. New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Henley, N. M. (1995). Body politics revisited: What do we know today? In Kalbfleisch, P. J. & Cody, M. J. (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 2761). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hess, U. (2017). Emotion Categorization. In Lefebvre, C. & Cohen, H. (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (2nd ed., pp. 107126). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2007). When two do the same it might not mean the same: The perception of emotional expressions shown by men and women. In Hess, U. & Philippot, P. (Eds.), Group dynamics and emotional expression (pp. 3352). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2008). The role of perceived emotion in first impressions. In Ambady, N. & Skowronski, J. J. (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 234254). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2009). Inter-group misunderstandings in emotion communication. In Demoulin, S., Leyens, J.-P., & Dovidio, J. F. (Eds.), Intergroup misunderstandings: Impact of divergent social realities (pp. 85100). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Simard, A., Stevenson, M. T., & Kleck, R. E. (2012). Smiling and sad wrinkles: Age-related changes in the face and the perception of emotions and intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 13771380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., Beaupré, M. G., & Cheung, N. (2002). Who to whom and why: Cultural differences and similarities in the function of smiles. In Abel, M. & Ceia, C. H. (Eds.), An empirical reflection on the smile (pp. 187216). New York: The Edwin Mellen Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. E. (2000). The influence of expression intensity, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 265283. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006623213355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., Blaison, C., & Dandeneau, S. (2017). The impact of rewards on empathic accuracy and emotional mimicry. Motivation and Emotion, 41(1), 107112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9590-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2016). The impact of context on the perception of emotions. In Abell, C. & Smith, J. (Eds.), The expression of emotion: Philosophical, psychological, and legal perspectives (pp. 199218). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2017). The social signal value of emotions: The role of contextual factors in social inferences drawn from emotion displays. In Russell, J. A. & Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 375392). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2019). The Emotion-Based Inferences in Context (EBIC) Model. In The social nature of emotion expression (pp. 1–5). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, Huppertz, U., Mauersberger, D., H., & Kastendieck, T. (2023). Wrinkles are neither beautiful nor nice: The effect of facial wrinkles on person perception and interpersonal closeness. Manuscript submitted for publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, U., & Kafetsios, K. (2022). Infusing context into emotion perception impacts emotion decoding accuracy: A truth and bias model. Experimental Psychology, 68, 285294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: Implicit prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychological Science, 14, 640643. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1478.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchings, P. B., & Haddock, G. (2008). Look Black in anger: The role of implicit prejudice in the categorization and perceived emotional intensity of racially ambiguous faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 14181420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). Vocal expression of affect. In Harrigan, J. A., Rosenthal, R., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), The new handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 65135). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastendieck, T., Mauersberger, H., Blaison, C., Ghalib, J., & Hess, U. (2020). Laughing at funerals and frowning at weddings: Top-down influences of context-driven social judgments on emotional mimicry. Acta Psychologica, 212, 103195.Google ScholarPubMed
Keating, C. F., Mazur, A., & Segall, M. H. (1981). A cross-cultural exploration of physiognomic traits of dominance and happiness. Ethology and Sociobiology, 2, 4148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, C. F., Mazur, A., Segall, M. H., et al. (1981). Culture and the perception of social dominance from facial expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 615625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A. (2004). PERSON: A general model of interpersonal perception. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 265280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirouac, G., & Hess, U. (1999). Group membership and the decoding of nonverbal behavior. In Philippot, P., Feldman, R., & Coats, E. (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 182210). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kleinke, C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 78100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02281954CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korthase, K. M., & Trenholme, I. (1982). Perceived age and perceived physical attractiveness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54(3 suppl.), 12511258. http://10.2466/pms.1982.54.3c.1251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaFrance, M., & Hecht, M. A. (1995). Why smiles generate leniency. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 207214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The Chameleon Effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 543554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.4.543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lass, N. J., Hughes, K. R., Bowyer, M. D., Waters, L. T., & Bourne, V. T. (1976). Speaker sex identification from voiced, whispered, and filtered isolated vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59(3), 675678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavater, J. C. (1804). Essays on physiognomy: For the promotion of the knowledge and the love of mankind (Vol. 1). London: C. Whittingham.Google Scholar
Likowski, K. U., Mühlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008). Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 10651072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Siegel, E. H., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). The hundred-year emotion war: Are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on Lench, Flores, and Bench (2011). Psychological Bulletin, 139, 255263. http://10.1037/a0029038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linville, S. E. (1996). The sound of senescence. Journal of Voice, 10(2), 190200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0892-1997(96)80046-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macrae, C. N., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C., & Mason, M. F. (2002). Are you looking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Psychological Science, 13, 460464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnée, M. J. C. M., Stekelenburg, J. J., Kemner, C., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Similar facial electromyographic responses to faces, voices, and body expressions. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 18, 369372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32801776e6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malatesta, C. Z., Izard, C. E., Culver, C., & Nicolich, M. (1987). Emotion communication skills in young, middle-aged, and older women. Psychology and Aging, 2, 193203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2015). Can we undo our first impressions? The role of reinterpretation in reversing implicit evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(6), 823849. http://10.1037/pspa0000021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221232). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion, 5, 119124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mauersberger, H., Kastendieck, T. M., Hetmann, A., Schöll, A., & Hess, U. (2022). The different shades of laughter: When do we laugh and when do we mimic other’s laughter? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 1863, 20210188.Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. Jr. (1973). Leadership and communication in young chimpanzees. In Menzel, J. E. W. (Ed.), Precultural primate behavior (pp. 192225). Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. Jr. (1974). A group of young chimpanzees in a one-acre field. In Schrier, A. M. & Stollnitz, F. (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (pp. 83153). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1993). A cross-cultural comparison of impressions created by age-related variations in gait. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17(1), 5568. http://10.1007/BF00987008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motley, M. T., & Camden, C. T. (1988). Facial expression of emotion: A comparison of posed expressions versus spontaneous expressions in an interpersonal communications setting. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 52, 122. http://10.1080/10570318809389622CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a predictor of later military rank. Social Forces, 74, 823850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., & Brauer, M. (2012). Social functionality of human emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 259285. http://10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkinson, B. (1999). Relations and dissociations between appraisal and emotion ratings of reasonable and unreasonable anger and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 347385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2001). Putting appraisal in context. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 173186). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reis, H. T., Wilson, I. M., Monestere, C., & Bernstein, S. (1990). What is smiling is beautiful and good. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20(3), 259267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by gender: How gender inequality persists in the modern world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 161200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J., & Evdokas, A. (2004). Appraisals cause experienced emotions: Experimental evidence. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J., Spindel, M. S., & Jose, P. E. (1990). Appraisals of emotion-eliciting events: Testing a theory of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5), 899915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2019). Affective pragmatics extended: From natural to overt expressions of emotions. In The social nature of emotion expression (pp. 49–81). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A., Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2022). Emotional expressions as appeals to recipients. Emotion, 22, 18561868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1997). Profiles of emotion-antecedent appraisal: Testing theoretical predictions across cultures. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 113150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Grandjean, D. (2008). Facial expressions allow inference of both emotions and their components. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 789801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, S., Christensen, A., Häußinger, F. B., et al. (2013). Show me how you walk and I tell you how you feel: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study on emotion perception based on human gait. NeuroImage, No Pagination Specified. http://10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.078Google Scholar
Schuller, B., Batliner, A., Steidl, S., & Seppi, D. (2011). Recognising realistic emotions and affect in speech: State of the art and lessons learnt from the first challenge. Speech Communication, 53(9–10), 10621087. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.01.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Barnes, J., & David, A. S. (1999). An investigation into the perception of dominance from schematic faces: A study using the World-Wide Web. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 31, 341346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Showers, C., & Cantor, N. (1985). Social cognition: A look at motivated strategies. Annual Review of Psychology, 36(1), 275305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, N., Bard, C., Dadouchi, F., et al. (1992). Posture and elderly persons: Evidence for deficits in the central integrative mechanisms. In Tutorials in motor behavior, 2 (pp. 917931). Oxford: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Thibault, P., Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2006). The effect of group-identification on emotion recognition: the case of cats and basketball players. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 676683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, G. R. (1943). The effect upon judgements of personality traits of varying a single factor in a photograph. Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 127148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todorov, A., & Uleman, J. S. (2002). Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors’ faces: Evidence from a false recognition paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 10511065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, A., & Uleman, J. S. (2003). The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actors’ faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 549562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(33), 1165511660. www.pnas.org/content/105/33/11655.abstractCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Schalk, J., Fischer, A., Doosje, B. J., et al. (2011). Convergent and divergent responses to emotional displays of ingroup and outgroup. Emotion, 11(2), 286298. http://10.1037/a0022582CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walter, H. (2012). Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Empathy: Concepts, Circuits, and Genes. Emotion Review, 4(1), 917. http://emr.sagepub.com/content/4/1/9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webbink, P. (1986). The power of the eyes. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Weyers, P., Mühlberger, A., Kund, A., Hess, U., & Pauli, P. (2009). Modulation of facial reactions to avatar emotional faces by nonconscious competition priming. Psychophysiology, 46, 328335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window to the soul? Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Zebrowitz, L. A., Fellous, J., Mignault, A., & Andreoletti, C. (2003). Trait impressions as overgeneralized responses to adaptively significant facial qualities: Evidence from connectionist modeling. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 194215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2006). The ecological approach to person perception: Evolutionary roots and contemporary offshoots. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J. A., & Kendrick, D. T. (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 81114). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×