Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6b989bf9dc-cvxtj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-14T22:21:40.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Some Perspectives on Positive Feelings and Emotions

Positive Affect Facilitates Thinking and Problem Solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Alice M. Isen
Affiliation:
Cornell University
Antony S. R. Manstead
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Nico Frijda
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Amsterdam
Agneta Fischer
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

ABSTRACT

The work reviewed in this chapter indicates that positive affect facilitates careful, thorough thinking and problem solving, and promotes a flexible, responsive approach to situations that fosters new learning as well as utilization of existing knowledge. Evidence indicates that these processes also facilitate pro-social behavior and flexibility in social perception. In addition, research has shown that positive affect increases some kinds of motivation by influencing some of its components as well. Thus, the chapter argues for a conceptualization that integrates affect, cognition, and behavior/motivation (the traditional trichotomy of mind) and recognizes the fact that they mutually influence one another. The chapter argues against the common assumption that affect and cognition are separate, competing systems or approaches, and shows, instead, that they have mutual influence and are subject to similar processes. Likewise, it is shown that motives arising out of affect need not be more compelling than motives arising from other sources. The data suggest, further, that positive and negative affect, rather than being opposites, have distinct effects. A neuropsychological theory of the influence of positive affect on cognitive functioning is discussed, and the chapter calls attention to the potential benefits to be had from integrating the neuropsychological, behavioral, and cognitive levels of analysis in studying affect.

Type
Chapter
Information
Feelings and Emotions
The Amsterdam Symposium
, pp. 263 - 281
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashby, F. G.; Isen, A. M.; & Turken, A. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect and self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aspinwall, L. G., & Richter, L. (1999). Optimism and self-mastery predict more rapid disengagement from unsolvable tasks in the presence of alternatives. Motivation and Emotion, 23 (3), 221–245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 417–436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, R. A. (1984). Reducing organizational conflict: An incompatible response approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 272–279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barone, M. J.; Miniard, P. W.; & Romeo, J. B. (2000). The influence of positive mood on brand extension evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 386–400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1972). Social norms, feelings, and other factors affecting helping and altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 63–108). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Bless, H.; Clore, G. L.; Schwarz, N.; Golisano, V.; Rabe, C.; & Wolk, M. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Does a happy mood really lead to mindlessness?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665–679CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabib, S., & Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1999). Of genes, environment, and destiny. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnevale, P. J. D., & Isen, A. M. (1986). The influence of positive-affect and visual access on the discovery of integrative solutions in bilateral negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 1–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, M. R.; Steinberg, J.; & Grev, R. (1980). Wanting to and having to help: Separate motivations for positive mood and guilt induced helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 181–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D. (1993). Attentional consequences of outcome-related motivational states: Congruent, incongruent, and focusing effects. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 65–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F.; Gaertner, S. L.; Isen, A. M.; & Lowrance, R. (1995). Group representations and intergroup bias: Positive affect, similarity, and group size. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 856–865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F.; Gaertner, S. L.; Isen, A. M.; Rust, M.; & Guerra, P. (1998). Positive affect, cognition, and the reduction of intergroup bias. In C. Sedikides, J. Schopler, and C. A. Insko (Eds.), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior (pp. 337–366). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum
Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (In press). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology
Estrada, C. A.; Isen, A. M.; & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: Affective influences on interpersonal behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 1–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions?Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heller, W. (1997). Emotion. In M. Banich (Ed.), Neuropsychology: The neural bases of mental function. New York: Houghton Mifflin
Ilgen, D. R.; Nebeker, D. M.; & Pritchard, R. D. (1981). Expectancy theory measures: An empirical comparison in an experimental situation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 28, 189–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1970). Success, failure attention and reactions to others: The warm glow of success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 107–112Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes and social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 203–253). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Isen, A. M. (1990). The influence of positive and negative affect on cognitive organization: Implications for development. In N. Stein, B. Leventhal, and T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and biological processes in the development of emotion (pp. 75–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis and J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 261–277). New York: Guilford
Isen, A. M. (1999). On the relationship between affect and creative problem solving. In S. Russ (Ed.), Affect, creative experience, and psychological adjustment (pp. 3–17). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis
Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis and J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, (2d ed. pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford
Isen, A. M. (2002a). A role for neuropsychology in understanding the facilitating influence of positive affect on social behavior and cognitive processes. In C. R. Snyder and S. J. Lopez, (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Isen, A. M. (2002b). Missing in action in the AIM: Positive affect's facilitation of cognitive flexibility, innovation, and problem solving. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 57–65Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (In press). Positive affect as a source of human strength. In L. Aspinwall and U. Staudinger (Eds.), The psychology of human strengths
Isen, A. M.; Christianson, M.; & Labroo, A. A. (2002). The nature of the task influences whether positive affect facilitates task performance. Manuscript. Cornell University
Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206–1217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M.; Daubman, K. A.; & Gorgoglione, J. M. (1987). The influence of positive affect on cognitive organization: Implications for education. In R. E. Snow and M. J. Farr (Eds.), Aptitude, learning, and instruction (pp. 143–164). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Isen, A. M.; Daubman, K. A.; & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., & Geva, N. (1987). The influence of positive affect on acceptable level of risk: The person with a large canoe has a large worry. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 145–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M.; Johnson, M. M. S.; Mertz, E.; & Robinson, F. G. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word association. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). The effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 384–388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M.; Niedenthal, P.; & Cantor, N. (1992). The influence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 65–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M.; Nygren, T. E.; & Ashby, F. G. (1988). The influence of positive affect on the perceived utility of gains and losses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 710–717CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., & Patrick, R. (1983). The influence of positive feelings on risk taking: When the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 194–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Reeve, J. M. (2002). Positive affect promotes intrinsic motivation. Manuscript. Cornell University
Isen, A. M; Rosenzweig, A. S.; & Young, M. J. (1991). The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making, 11, 221–227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M.; Shalker, T. E.; Clark, M.; & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: A cognitive loop?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1–12CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M.; & Simmonds, S. F. (1978). The effect of feeling good on a helping task that is incompatible with good mood. Social Psychology, 41, 346–349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isom, J., & Heller, W. (1999). Neurobiology of extraversion: Pieces of the puzzle still missing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn, B., & Isen, A. M. (1993). The influence of positive affect on variety-seeking among safe, enjoyable products. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 257–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraiger, K.; Billings, R. S.; & Isen, A. M. (1989). The influence of positive-affective states on task perceptions and satisfaction. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 12–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labroo, A. A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). Positive affect cues fairness in consumer judgment. Manuscript, Cornell University
Labroo, A. A.; Isen, A. M.; & Shanteau, J. (2000). The influence of positive affect on strategic decision making in “Prisoner's dilemma” situations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Judgment/Decision Making, New Orleans
Lee, A., & Sternthal, B. (1999). The effects of positive mood on memory. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellers, B. A. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (6), 910–924CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A. (1996). How we think about cognition, emotion, and biology in psychopathology. Psychophysiology, 33, 615–628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nygren, T. E.; Isen, A. M.; Taylor, P. J.; & Dulin, J. (1996). The influence of positive affect on the decision rule in risk situations: Focus on outcome (avoidance of loss) rather than probability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66, 59–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A. M.; Roberts, A. C.; Hodges, J. R.; Summers, B. A.; Polkey, C. E.; & Robbins, T. W. (1993). Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. Brain, 116, 1159–1175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, M. B., & Aspinwall, L. G. (1998). Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 99–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffenbauer, A. (1974). Effects of observer's emotional state on judgments of the emotional state of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 31–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgment (pp. 55–71). Oxford: Pergamon
Smith, S. M., & Shaffer, D. R. (1991). The effects of good moods on systematic processing: “Willing but not able, or able but not willing?”Motivation and Emotion, 15, 243–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1993). Affect and managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 304–331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 248–257CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D., & Russell, M. L. (1983). Differential aspects of induced mood on the recall of positive, negative and neutral words. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 163–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., & Pomerantz, E. M. (1998). Resolving conflicts among self-evaluative motives: Positive experiences as a resource for overcoming defensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 53–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Urada, M., & Miller, N. (2000). The impact of positive mood and category importance on crossed categorization effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 417–433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D. A., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×