Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76dd75c94c-28gj6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T09:33:31.976Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - A general inference skill

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2015

Edward J. O'Brien
Affiliation:
University of New Hampshire
Anne E. Cook
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Robert F. Lorch, Jr
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, B. P. (1986). Referential and causal coherence in the story comprehension of children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 336–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, B. P. (1988). Reason inferences in the story comprehension of children and adults. Child Development, 59, 1426–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albrecht, J. E., & O’Brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1061–70.Google Scholar
Albrecht, J. E., & O’Brien, E. J. (1995). Goal processing and the maintenance of global coherence. In Lorch, R. F. & O’Brien, E. J. (eds.) Sources of Coherence in Reading (pp. 159–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Alexander, P. A., & The Disciplined Reading and Learning Research Laboratory (2012). Reading into the future: competence for the 21st century. Educational Psychologist, 47, 259–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In Guthrie, J. (ed.), Comprehension and Teaching: Research Reviews (pp. 77117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L. (1984). Children’s effective use of multiple standards for evaluating their comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 588–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, M. A., Dennis, M., & Haefele-Kalvaitis, J. (1996). The effects of knowledge availability and knowledge accessibility on coherence and elaborative inferencing in children from six to fifteen years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 216–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basche, P. (2001). Inferential questioning: effects on comprehension of narrative texts as a function of grade and timing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 521–29.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J. (2007). Recall in infancy: a neurodevelopmental account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 142–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P. J., & Lukowski, A. F. (2010). The memory is in the details: relations between memory for the specific features of events and long-term recall in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 506–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkeley, S., Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (2010). Reading comprehension instruction for students with learning disabilities, 1995–2006: a meta-analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 31, 423–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanc, N., Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., & Brouillet, D. (2008). Updating situation models: empirical data and simulations. Discourse Processes, 45, 103–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourg, T., Bauer, P., & van den Broek, P. (1997). Building the bridges: the development of event comprehension and representation. In van den Broek, P., Bauer, P., & Bourg, T. (eds.), Developmental Spans in Event Comprehension and Representation: Bridging Fictional and Actual Events (pp. 385407). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowyer-Crane, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2005). Assessing children’s inference generation: what do tests of reading comprehension measure? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 189201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bus, A. G., & Neuman, S. B. (2009). Multimedia and Literacy Development: Improving Achievement for Young Learners. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (1999). Inference making ability and its relation to comprehension failure in young children. Reading and Writing, 11(5–6), 489503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(4), 683–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K. Oakhill, J. V., Barnes, M. A., & Bryant, P. E. (2001). Comprehension skill, inference making ability, and the relation to knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 29, 850–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Elbro, C. (2003). The ability to learn new word meanings from context by school-age children with and without language comprehension difficulties. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 681–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: the influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 671–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casteel, M. A. (1993). Effects of inference necessity and reading goal on children’s inferential generation. Developmental Psychology, 29, 346–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casteel, M. A., & Simpson, G. B. (1991). Textual coherence and the development of inferential generation skills. Journal of Research in Reading, 14, 116–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Common Core State Standards Initiative. (2010). Common core state standards for English language arts and literacy in history/social studies, science, and technical subjects. Washington, DC: Council of Chief State School Officers and National Governors Association. Retrieved from www.corestandards.org/Google Scholar
Cook, A. E., Halleran, J. G., & O’Brien, E. J. (1998). What is readily available during reading? A memory-based view of text processing. Discourse Processes, 26(2&3), 109–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, A. E., Lassonde, K. A., Splinter, A., Guéraud, S., Stiegler-Balfour, J. J., & O’Brien, E. J. (2013). The role of relevance in the activation and instantiation of predictive inferences. Language and Cognitive Processes, 29, 244–57.Google Scholar
Cook, A. E., & O’Brien, E. J. (2014). Knowledge activation, integration, and validation during narrative text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 51, 2649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N. (2010). Multiple concurrent thoughts: the meaning and developmental neuropsychology of working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(5), 447–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromley, J., & Azevedo, R. (2007). Testing and refining the Direct and Inferential Mediation Model of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 311–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duke, N. K. (2004). The case for informational text. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 40–5.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C., & Buch-Iversen, I. (2013). Activation of background knowledge for inference making: effects on reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 435–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., & Conway, A. R. A. (1998). Working memory and comprehension. In Logie, R. H. & Gilhooly, K. J. (eds.), Working Memory and Thinking (pp. 6792). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fricke, S., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., Haley, A. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2013). Efficacy of language intervention in the early years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(3), 280–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: a theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language Comprehension as Structure Building. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerrig, R. J., & O’Brien, E. J. (2005). The scope of memory-based processing. Discourse Processes, 39, 225–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. R., & Varnhagen, C. K. (1986). Memory for embedded and sequential episodes in stories. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 401–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (1998). Facilitating knowledge-based inferences in less-skilled readers. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 149–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendeou, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., White, M. J., & van den Broek, P. (2008). Children’s inference generation across different media. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 259–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., Lynch, J., Broek, P., Espin, C., White, M., & Kremer, K. (2005). Developing successful readers: building early comprehension skills through television viewing and listening. Early Childhood Education Journal, 33(2), 91–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., Smith, E. R., & O’Brien, E. J. (2013). Updating during reading comprehension: why causality matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 854–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2005). The effects of readers’ misconceptions on comprehension of scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 235–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2007). The effects of prior knowledge and text structure on comprehension processes during reading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 35(7), 1567–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., Helder, A., & Karlsson, A. K. J. (2014). A cognitive view of reading comprehension: implications for reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 29(1), 1016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M., & Lynch, J. (2007). Preschool and early elementary comprehension: skill development and strategy interventions. In McNamara, D. S. (ed.), Reading Comprehension Strategies: Theories, Interventions, and Technologies. (pp. 2745). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M., & Lynch, J. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: the independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 765–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1993). Information accretion and reduction in text processing: inferences. Special issue: Inference generation during text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 161–2, 193202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Kintsch, E. (2005). Comprehension. In Paris, S. G. and Stahl, S. A. (eds.), Current Issues in Reading Comprehension and Assessment (pp. 7192). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lassonde, K. A., & O’Brien, E. J. (2009). Contextual specificity in the activation of predictive inferences. Discourse Processes, 46, 426–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lasson de, K. A., & O’Brien, E. J. (2013). Occupational stereotypes: activation of male bias in a gender neutral world. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 387–96.Google Scholar
Lea, R. B. (1995). Online evidence for elaborative logical inferences in text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1469–82.Google Scholar
Lea, R. B. (1998). Logical inference and comprehension: how mental logic and text processing theories need each other. In Braine, M. D. S. & O’Brien, D. P. (eds.), Mental Logic (pp. 6378). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lea, R. B., Kayser, P., Mulligan, E. J., & Myers, J. (2002) Do readers make inferences about conversational topics? Memory & Cognition, 30, 945–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linderholm, T., & van den Broek, P. (2002). The effects of reading purpose and working memory capacity on the processing of expository text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 778–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Reichle, E. D., & Gao, D. -G. (2013). Using reinforcement learning to examine dynamic attention allocation during reading. Cognitive Science, 37, 1507–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, D. L., & Chong, J. L. (2001). Comprehension skill and global coherence: A paradoxical picture of poor comprehenders’ abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(6), 1424–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Long, D. L., Oppy, B. J., & Seely, M. R. (1994). Individual differences in the time course of inferential processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(6), 1456–70.Google Scholar
Lorch, E. P., & Sanchez, R. P. (1997). Children’s memory for televised events. In van den Broek, P. W., Bauer, P. J. & Bourg, T. (eds.), Developmental Spans in Event Comprehension and Representation: Bridging Fictional and Actual Events. (pp. 271–91). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. S., & van den Broek, P. (2007). Understanding the glue of narrative structure: children’s on- and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cognitive Development, 22, 323–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Kremer, K. E., Kendeou, P., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. (2008). The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29, 327–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. (1996). Generating predictive inferences while viewing a movie. Discourse Processes, 22, 199224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Kopp, K., McNerney, M. W., Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2012). Aging and perceived event structure as a function of modality. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 264–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magliano, J. P., Loschky, L. C., Clinton, J., & Larson, A. M. (2013). Is reading the same as viewing? An exploration of the similarities and differences between processing text- and visually based narratives. In Miller, B., Cutting, L., and McCardle, P. (eds.), Unraveling the Behavioral, Neurobiological, and Genetic Components of Reading Comprehension (pp. 7890). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Radvansky, G. A., & Copeland, D. E. (2007). Beyond language comprehension: situation models as a form or autobiographical memory. In Schmalhofer, F. & Perfetti, C. (eds.), Higher Level Language Processes in the Brain: Inference and Comprehension Processes (pp. 379–91). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McGee, A., & Johnson, H. (2003). The effect of inference training on skilled and less skilled comprehenders. Educational Psychology, 23, 4959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., & Blake, R. G. K. (2009). Rethinking reading comprehension instruction: a comparison of instruction for strategies and content approaches. Reading Research Quarterly, 44(3), 218–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99, 440–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMaster, K. L., van den Broek, P., Espin, C. A., White, M. J., Rapp, D. N., Kendeou, P., Bohn-Gettler, C. M., & Carlson, S. (2012). Making the right connections: differential effects of reading intervention for subgroups of comprehenders. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1), 100–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, D.S. (2001). Reading both high-coherence and low-coherence texts: effects of text sequence and prior knowledge. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. (2009). Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension. In Ross, B. (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 297384). New York: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moshman, D. (2004). From inference to reasoning: the construction of rationality. Thinking & Reasoning, 10, 221–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., & O’Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26(2–3), 131–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(2), 233–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K. (2005). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties. In Snowling, M. & Hulme, C. (eds.), The Science of Reading: A Handbook (pp. 248–65). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Neuman, S. B. (1988). The displacement effect: assessing the relation between television viewing and reading performance. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 414–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J. (1984). Inferential and memory skills in children’s comprehension of stories. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 3139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(4), 443–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J., Hartt, J., & Samols, D. (2005). Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing, 18(7–9), 657–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, E. J. (1987). Antecedent search processes and the structure of text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 278–90.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, E. J., & Albrecht, J. E., (1992). Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 777–84.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, E. J., Cook, A. E., & Guéraud, S., (2010). Accessibility of outdated information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 979–91.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, E. J., Cook, A. E., & Peracchi, K. A. (2004). Updating a situation model: a reply to Zwaan and Madden (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 289–91.Google Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., & Myers, J. L. (1987). The role of causal connections in the retrieval of text. Memory & Cognition, 15, 419–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, E. J., & Myers, J. L. (1999). Text comprehension: a view from the bottom up. In Goldman, S. R., Graesser, A. C., & van den Broek, P. (eds.), Narrative Comprehension, Causality, and Coherence: Essays in Honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 3653). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., Shank, D. A., Myers, J. L., & Rayner, K. (1988). Elaborative inferences during reading: do they occur online? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 410–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Omanson, R. C., Warren, W. H., & Trabasso, T. (1978). Goals, inferential comprehension, and recall of stories by children. Discourse Processes, 1, 337–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paris, S. G., & Lindauer, B. K. (1976). The role of inference in children’s comprehension and memory for sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 217–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paris, S. G., Lindauer, B. K., & Cox, G. (1977). The development of inferential comprehension. Child Development, 48, 1728–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paris, S. G., & Upton, L. R. (1976). Children’s memory for inferential relationships in prose. Child Development, 47, 660–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peracchi, K. A., & O’Brien, E. J. (2004). Character profiles and the activation of predictive inferences. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1044–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 2237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pezdek, K., Lehrer, A., & Simon, S. (1984). The relationship between reading and cognitive processing of television and radio. Child Development, 55, 2072–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressley, M., Graham, S., & Harris, K. (2006). The state of educational intervention research as viewed through the lens of literacy intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for Understanding: Toward a Research and Development Program in Reading Comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.Google Scholar
Rapp, D. N., van den Broek, P., McMaster, K. L., Kendeou, P., & Espin, C. A. (2007). Higher-order comprehension processes in struggling readers: a perspective for research and intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 289312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St. George, M., Mannes, S., & Hoffman, J. E. (1997). Individual differences in inference generation: An ERP analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 776–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, M., & Lea, R. B. (2012) Inference and reasoning in discourse comprehension. In Schmid, H.-J. and Geeraerts, D. (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Pragmatics (pp. 85119). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, M., & Ritchot, K. (1996). Individual differences in inference validation. Memory & Cognition, 24, 733–43.Google Scholar
Stafura, J. Z., & Perfetti, C. A. (2014). Word-to-text integration: message level and lexical level influences in ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 64, 4153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L., & O’Connor, R. (2009). The role of working memory and fluency practice on the reading comprehension of students who are dysfluent readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(6), 548–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second to fifth grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Nickels, M. (1992). The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes, 15(3), 249–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: the development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In van den Broek, P., Bauer, P. W., & Bourg, T. (eds.), Developmental Spans in Event Comprehension and Representation (pp. 321–42). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., Kendeou, P., Carlson, S., & White, M. J. (2011). When a reader meets a text: the role of standards of coherence in reading comprehension. In McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J., & Schraw, G. (eds.), Relevance Instructions and Goal-Focusing in Text Learning (pp. 123–40). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Fletcher, C. R., & Risden, K. (1993). Investigations of inferential processes in reading: a theoretical and methodological integration. Discourse Processes, 16, 169–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., Kremer, K., Lynch, J. S., Butler, J., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. P. (2005). Assessment of comprehension abilities in young children. In Paris, S. & Stahl, S. (eds.), Children’s Reading Comprehension and Assessment (pp. 107–30). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & White, M. J. (2009). Cognitive processes during reading: implications for the use of multimedia to foster reading comprehension. In Bus, A. G. & Neuman, S. B. (eds.), Multimedia and Literacy Development: Improving Achievement for Young Learners (pp. 5773). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, R., Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & Cognition, 29(8), 1081–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, E.P., & Thurlow, R. (1996). Children’s and adults’ memory for television stories: the role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child Development, 67, 3010–28.Google ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P., Rapp, D. N., & Kendeou, P. (2005). Integrating memory-based and constructionist approaches in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye-Hartmann, E. (1995). The role of readers’ standards for coherence in the generation of inferences during reading. In Lorch, R. F. & O’Brien, E. J. (eds.), Sources of Coherence in Text Comprehension (pp. 353–73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W. (1989). Causal reasoning and inference making in judging the importance of story statements. Child Development, 60, 286–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Broek, P. W. (1990). The causal inference maker: towards a process model of inference generation in text comprehension. In Balota, D. A., d’Arcais, G. B. Flores & Rayner, K. (eds.), Comprehension Processes in Reading (pp. 423–46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: the development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In van den Broek, P. W., Bauer, P. J. & Bourg, T. (eds.), Developmental Spans in Event Comprehension and Representation: Bridging Fictional and Actual Events (pp. 321–42). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W., Helder, A., & Van Leijenhorst, L. (2013). Sensitivity to structural centrality: developmental and individual differences in reading comprehension skills. In Britt, M. A., Goldman, S. R. & Rouet, J.-F. (eds.), Reading: From Words to Multiple Texts (pp. 132–46). New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W., Kendeou, P., Lousberg, S., & Visser, G. (2011). Preparing for reading comprehension: fostering text comprehension skills in preschool and early elementary school children. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 4(1), 259–68.Google Scholar
Wenner, J. (2004). Preschoolers’ comprehension of goal structure in narratives. Memory, 12(2), 192202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., Lauer, K. D., Stafford, K. B., DeSisto, L. A., & deCani, J. S. (2005). Expository text comprehension in the primary grade classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 538–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. (1988). Effects of inference awareness training on poor reading comprehension. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2, 3345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×