Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T06:23:45.976Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Interaction between leopard and monkeys

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2009

W. Scott McGraw
Affiliation:
Ohio State University
Klaus Zuberbühler
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
Ronald Noë
Affiliation:
Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Although predation is clearly a crucial factor in the evolution of primates its actual effects as a selective force are not well understood. Predation is thought to have affected various traits such as body size, group size and composition, vigilance, ecological niche, as well as vocal and reproductive behavior (van Schaik 1983, Cheney & Wrangham 1987, Cords 1990, Hill & Dunbar 1998, Stanford 1998, Uster & Zuberbühler 2001). However, there are reasons to remain cautious about many of the proposed relationships. In particular, little is known about the hunting pressure exerted by the various primate predators and the selective pressure they impose on a primate community. Nevertheless, predation is often treated as a homogeneous evolutionary force even though predators differ considerably in their hunting behavior. For instance in the Taï forest, monkeys are hunted by chimpanzees Pan troglodytes, crowned eagles Stephanoaetus coronatus, and leopards Panthera pardus. Predatory chimpanzees locate monkey groups by acoustic cues and hunt for individuals in the high canopy (Boesch & Boesch-Achermann 2000). Not surprisingly, the presence of chimpanzees reliably elicits cryptic behavior in nearby monkeys (Zuberbühler et al. 1999). Crowned eagles, in contrast, hunt by sweeping through the canopy to surprise their prey (Gautier-Hion & Tutin 1988, Shultz 2001) and their discovery typically elicits loud and conspicuous alarm calling and sometimes even mobbing behavior (Zuberbühler 2000b). Because they differ fundamentally in their hunting strategies, the selective force of chimpanzees, leopards, and eagles – as predators – is not homogeneous.

Type
Chapter
Information
Monkeys of the Taï Forest
An African Primate Community
, pp. 133 - 154
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bailey, T. N. (1993). The African Leopard. New York, Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Begon, M., Harper, J. L.et al. (1996). Ecology. Oxford, Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bodendorfer, T. (1994). Zur Ernährungsbiologie des Leoparden Panthera pardus und des Löwen Panthera leo im Comoé- und Marahoué Nationalpark, Elfenbeinküste – Eine Untersuchung anhand von Kotproben. M.Sc. Thesis, University of Vienna, Australia.
Boesch, C. (1991). The effects of leopard predation on grouping patterns in forest chimpanzees. Behaviour, 117(3–4), 220–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78, 547–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest. Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1981). Selective forces affecting the predator alarm calls of vervet monkeys. Behaviour, 76, 25–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Wrangham, R. W. (1987). Predation. In Primate Societies, ed. Smuts, B., D. L. Cheney, , R. M. Seyfarth, , R. W. Wrangham, and Struhsaker, T. T., Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 227–39.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (1990). Vigilance and mixed-species association of some East African forest monkeys. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26(4), 297–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cresswell, W. (1994). Song as a pursuit-deterrent signal, and its occurrence relative to other anti-predation behaviors of skylark (Alauda arvensis) on attack by merlins (Falco columbarius). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 34(3), 217–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dind, F., Jenny, D. and Boesch, C. (1996). Ecologie et prédation du leopard en forêt de Taï (Côte d'Ivoire). Rapport d'activité 1992–4, ed. J. F. Graf, M. Zinsstag and J. Zinsstag. Abidjan, Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques en Côte d'Ivoire, 22–6.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate Social Systems. New York: Comstock Publishing Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckardt, W. (2002). Nischenvergleich der Grossen Weissnasenmeerkatze (Cercopithecus nictitans) und der Dianameerkatze (Cercopithecus diana) im Taï Nationalpark (Elfenbeinküste) under besonderer Berücksichtigung interspezifischer Konkurrenz. M.Sc. Thesis, University of Leipzig, Germany.
Gautier-Hion, A. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1988). Simultaneous attack by adult males of a polyspecific troop of monkeys against a crowned hawk eagle. Folia Primatologica, 51, 149–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimes, K. (2002). Hunted. Scientific American, 2338, 34–7.Google Scholar
Hamilton, P. H. (1981). The leopard Panthera pardus and cheetah Acinonyx jubatus in Kenya; ecology, status, conservation and management. The African Wildlife Leadership Foundation, and the Government of Kenya.
Hart, J. A., Katembo, M. and Punga, K. (1996). Diet, prey selection and ecological relations of leopard and golden cat in the Ituri forest, Zaire. African Journal of Ecology, 34, 364–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, R. A. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). An evaluation of the roles of predation rate and predation risk as selective pressures on primate grouping patterns. Behaviour, 135, 411–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppe-Dominik, B. (1984). Etude du spectre des proies de la panthère, Panthera pardus, dans le Parc National de Taï en Côte d'Ivoire. Mammalia, 48, 477–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isbell, L. (1994). Predation on primates: ecological patterns and evolutionary consequences. Evolutionary Anthropology, 3, 61–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, C. H. (1998). Testing the predation hypothesis for vertebrate sociality: prospects and pitfalls. Behaviour, 135(4), 389–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenny, D. (1996). Spatial organization of leopards (Panthera pardus) in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast: Is rain forest habitat a tropical haven?Journal of Zoology, 240, 427–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenny, D. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Hunting behavior in West African forest leopards. African Journal of Ecology, 43, 197–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karanth, K. U. and Sunquist, M. E. (1995). Prey selection by tiger, leopard, and dhole in tropical forests. Journal of Animal Ecology, 64, 439–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karanth, K. U. and Sunquist, M. E. (2000). Behavioural correlates of predation by tiger (Panthera tigris), leopard (Panthera pardus) and dhole (Cuon alpinus) in Nagarahole, India. Journal of Zoology, 250, 255–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitchener, A. (1991). The Natural History of the World's Cats. Ithaca: Cornell University.Google Scholar
McGraw, W. S. (1998). Comparative locomotion and habitat use of six monkeys in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 105, 493–510.3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGraw, W. S. (2000). Positional behavior of Cercopithecus petaurista. International Journal of Primatology, 21, 157–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oates, J. F., Whitesides, G. H., Davies, A. G.et al. (1990). Determinants of variation in tropical forest primate biomass – new evidence from West Africa. Ecology, 71, 328–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plavcan, J. M. and Schaik, C. P. (1992). Intrasexual competition and canine dimorphism in anthropoid primates. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 87(4), 461–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ray, J. C. and Sunquist, M. E. (2001). Trophic relations in a community of African rainforest carnivores. Oecologia, 127(3), 395–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marier, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210, 801–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shultz, S. (2001). Notes on interactions between monkeys and African crowned eagles in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Folia Primatologica, 72, 248–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanford, C. B. (1998). Predation and male bonds in primate societies. Behaviour, 135, 513–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoddard, P. K. (1999). Predation enhances complexity in the evolution of electric fish signals. Nature, 400, 254–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uphyrkina, O., Johnson, W. E., Quigley, H.et al. (2001). Phylogenetics, genome diversity and origin of modern leopard, Panthera pardus. Molecular Ecology, 10, 2617–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uster, D. and Zuberbühler, K. (2001). The functional significance of Diana monkey “clear” calls. Behaviour, 138(6), 741–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaik, C. P. (1983). Why are diurnal primates living in groups?Behaviour, 87(1–2), 120–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000a). Causal cognition in a non-human primate: field playback experiments with Diana monkeys. Cognition, 76, 195–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000b). Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59(1), 209–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000c). Interspecific semantic communication in two forest monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 267(1444), 713–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000d). Referential labelling in Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59(5), 917–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2001). Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell's guenons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 414–22.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2002). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63(2), 293–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. and Jenny, D. (2002). Leopard predation and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 43, 873–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K., Jenny, D. and Bshary, R. (1999). The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology, 105(6), 477–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K., Noë R, . and Seyfarth, R. M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 53, 589–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×