Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T17:44:30.524Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - The Evolution of Linguistic Communication: Piagetian Insights

from Part III - Language and Communicative Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Nancy Budwig
Affiliation:
Clark University, Massachusetts
Elliot Turiel
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Philip David Zelazo
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aronova, E. (2007). Karl Popper and Lamarckism. Biological Theory, 2(1), 3751.Google Scholar
Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: Behavioural inheritance in evolution. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Seghier, M. L., Baquero, S., Estévez, A., Lozano, A., Devlin, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2009). An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature, 461, 983986.Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P., Courrége, P., & Danchin, A. (1973). A theory of the epigenesis of neuronal networks by selective stabilization of synapses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 70, 29742978.Google Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489558.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Dias, B. G., & Ressler, K. J., (2014). Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 8996.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, D. (2014). The instruction of imagination: Language and its evolution as a communication technology. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 105125). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2000). From cultural selection to genetic selection: A framework for the evolution of language. Selection, 1, 3355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2010). Plasticity and canalization in the evolution of linguistic communication, In Larson, R. K., Déprez, V., & Yamakido, H. (Eds.), The evolution of human language (pp. 135147). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2014). Why we need to move from gene-culture co-evolution to culturally-driven co-evolution. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 1530). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473, 7982. doi: 10.1038/nature09923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, G. (1974). The problem of molecular recognition by a selective system. In Ayala, F. & Dobzhansky, T. (Eds.), Studies in the philosophy of biology (pp. 4556). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429492. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999094XGoogle Scholar
Franklin, T. B., Russig, H., Weiss, I. C., Gräff, J., Linder, N., Michalon, A., Vizi, S., & Mansuy, I. M., (2010). Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 408415.Google Scholar
Gapp, K., Jawaid, A., Sarkies, P., Bohacek, J., Pelczar, P., Prados, J., Farinelli, L., Miska, E., & Mansuy, I. M. (2014). Implication of sperm RNAs in transgenerational inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 667669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginsburg, S., & Jablonka, E. (2014). Memory, imagination and the evolution of modern language. In Dor, D., Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (Eds.). The social origins of language (pp. 317324). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, R. (1935). Gen und Ausseneigenschaft. Zeitschrift für Induktive Abstammungs und Verebunglehre, 69, 38131.Google Scholar
Goodwin, B. (2009). Beyond the Darwinian Paradigm: Understanding Biological Forms. In Ruse, M. & Travis, J. (Eds.). Evolution: The first four billion years (pp. 299312). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 111.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. (2007). The developmental construction of heredity. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 808817,Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., Ginsburg, S., & Dor, D. (2012). The co-evolution of language and emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 21522159. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0117Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (1995). Epigenetic inheritance and evolution: The Lamarckian dimension. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2007a). Précis of “Evolution in four dimensions.” Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 353365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2007b). Bridging the gap: The developmental aspects of evolution. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 378392.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2014). Evolution in four dimensions (2nd edn.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka-Tavory, E. (1982). Genocopies and the evolution of interdependence. Evolutionary Theory, 6, 167170.Google Scholar
Laland, K., Uller, T., Feldman, M., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E., & Odling-Smee, J. (2014). Does evolutionary theory need a rethink? Yes, urgently! Nature, 514, 161164.Google Scholar
Longa, V. M. (2013). The evolution of the faculty of language from a Chomskyan perspective: Bridging linguistics and biology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 91, 148.Google Scholar
Lorenzo, G., & Longa, V. M. (2003). Minimizing the genes for grammar. The minimalist program as a biological framework for the study of language. Lingua, 113, 643657.Google Scholar
Manly, J. J., Byrd, D., Touradji, P., Sanchez, D., & Stern, Y. (2004). Literacy and cognitive change among ethnically diverse elders. International Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 4760.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Niemann, H-G. (2014). Karl Popper and the two new secrets of life. Tubingen, Germany: Mohr Siebeck.Google Scholar
Noble, D., Jablonka, E., Joyner, M. J., Müller, G. B., & Omholt, S. W. (2014). Evolution evolves: Physiology returns to centre stage. Journal of Physiology, 592, 22372244.Google Scholar
Palmer, A. R. (2009). Animal asymmetry. Current Biology, 19, R473R477.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1928, Trans. 1995). Genetic logic and sociology. In Smith, L. (Ed.), Piaget, J., Sociological Studies (pp. 184214). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge: An essay on the relations between organic regulations and cognitive processes. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1978). Behavior and evolution. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (Ed.). (1980). Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pigliucci, M., & Müller, G. B. (Eds.). (2010). Evolution: The extended synthesis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Puglia, M. H., Lillard, T. S., Morris, J. P., & Connelly, J. J. (2015). Epigenetic modification of the oxytocin receptor gene influences the perception of anger and fear in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 112, 33083313.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. G. (1953). The Baldwin effect. Evolution 7, 110117.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501504.Google Scholar
Steele, E. J., & Lloyd, S. S. (2015). Soma-to-germline feedback is implied by the extreme polymorphism at IGHV relative to MHC. BioEssays, 37, 557569. doi 10.1002/bies.201400213Google Scholar
Stern, S., Fridmann-Sirkis, Y., Braun, E., & Soen, Y. (2012). Epigenetically heritable alteration of fly development in response to toxic challenge. Cell Reports, 1, 528542.Google Scholar
Szyf, M. (2015). Nongenetic inheritance and transgenerational epigenetics. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 21, 134144.Google Scholar
Thaler, L., Arnott, S. R., & Goodale, M. A. (2011). Neural correlates of natural human echolocation in early and late blind echolocation experts. PLoS ONE, 6, e20162. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020162Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Boston, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2011). Human culture in evolutionary perspective. In Gelfand, M., Chiu, C.-y., & Hong, Y.-y (Eds.) Advances in Culture and Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 551). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vecchi, D., & Baravalle, L. (2015). A soul of truth in things erroneous: Popper’s “amateurish” evolutionary philosophy in light of contemporary biology. History and Philosophy of Life Sciences, 36, 525545. doi 10.1007/s40656-014-0047-5Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes, Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., & Souberman, E. (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Kozulin, A., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1975). The evolution of an evolutionist. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., Dymov, S., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×