Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T14:29:35.502Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Physiologically based models of binocular depth perception

from Part I - Depth processing and stereopsis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2011

Ning Qian
Affiliation:
Columbia University
Yongjie Li
Affiliation:
Columbia University
Laurence R. Harris
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Michael R. M. Jenkin
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Get access

Summary

Introduction

We perceive the world as three-dimensional. The inputs to our visual system, however, are only a pair of two-dimensional projections on the two retinal surfaces. As emphasized by Marr and Poggio (1976), it is generally impossible to uniquely determine the three-dimensional world from its two-dimensional retinal projections. How, then, do we usually perceive a well-defined three-dimensional environment? It has long been recognized that, since the world we live in is not random, the visual system has evolved and developed to take advantage of the world's statistical regularities, which are reflected in the retinal images. Some of these image regularities, termed depth cues, are interpreted by the visual system as depth. Numerous depth cues have been discovered. Many of them, such as perspective, shading, texture, motion, and occlusion, are present in the retina of a single eye, and are thus called monocular depth cues. Other cues are called binocular, as they result from comparing the two retinal projections. In the following, we will review our physiologically based models for three binocular depth cues: horizontal disparity (Qian, 1994; Chen and Qian, 2004), vertical disparity (Matthews et al., 2003), and interocular time delay (Qian and Andersen, 1994; Qian and Freeman, 2009). We have also constructed a model for depth perception from monocularly occluded regions (Assee and Qian, 2007), another binocular depth cue, but have omitted it here owing to space limitations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×