We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
The chapter begins with a review of the historical and current socio-political context for sexual minority and gender diverse (SMGD) individuals living in Nigeria, followed by relevant research on the associations between minority stress and well-being. Given the dearth of research on this topic, a large portion of this chapter focuses on recommendations for future research and practice for those interested in working with SMGD individuals living in Nigeria.
Human language is a magnificently complex cognitive process, integrating many of the processes we’ve already discussed. You must accurately perceive the letters or the sounds. You must link them to the intended memory representations of what those letters or sounds correspond to. Then you must make sense of the piece of language as a whole, in the context of a rich network of memories. Think of what goes unstated in the earlier joke. You must choose a meaning for “cutting” that is not about knives or standing in line but about insults. You must understand that a “clown” is not just a positive role of someone who entertains at children’s birthday parties and is a talented physical comedian in circuses but also someone who is rude or stupid. Finally, you have to see the humor in calling someone rude or stupid, but not so rude and stupid that they become famous for it.
The chapter begins with a review of the historical and current socio-political context for sexual minority and gender diverse (SMGD) individuals living in Türkiye followed by relevant research on the associations between minority stress and well-being. A particular focus is devoted to presenting data collected as part of the SMGD-MN study. The chapter concludes with recommendations for future psychological research with SMGD communities in Türkiye.
This introduction provides an overview of the SMGD-MN study, including information on the context to which it was developed and its theoretical underpinnings. Additionally, we provide key terminology and an overview of the chapters’ content.
The chapter begins with a review of the historical and current socio-political context for sexual minority and gender diverse (SMGD) individuals living in Austria, followed by relevant research on the associations between minority stress and well-being. A particular focus is devoted to presenting data collected as part of the SMGD-MN study. The chapter concludes with recommendations for future psychological research with SMGD communities in Austria.
In Chapter 1, we discussed the objections the behaviorists raised to the cognitive program. One of their concerns was the use of nonobservables in theory, for example, creating a theory of how memory works that includes representations such as short-term and long-term memory. No one can actually see or otherwise directly observe short-term memory, so how can we use it to explain human behavior? Cognitivists replied that they were going to use human behavior to test their models. But if so, it seems inevitable that their reasoning would end up being circular. They want to explain how humans behave, yet they plan to test whether the model is right using that same behavior.
In the previous chapter we considered the structure of our memories. What are the different kinds of memories? How are they organized? In this chapter and the next we will focus on the processes that turn experiences into memories and help us recall these memories later. Each chapter describes one of the two basic processes of memory: encoding and retrieval.
The chapter begins with a review of the historical and current socio-political context for sexual minority and gender diverse (SMGD) individuals living in India, followed by relevant research on the associations between minority stress and well-being. A particular focus is devoted to presenting data collected as part of the SMGD-MN study. The chapter concludes with recommendations for future psychological research with SMGD communities in India.
While our working memory system maintains information for short periods of time – seconds or minutes – we also need to maintain information over much longer periods: hours, days, weeks, months, and even years. Researchers call this long-term memory. Most questions people have about their own long-term memories are prompted when there’s a failure. Why can’t I remember that person’s name? Where did I put my glasses? What’s the answer to number 7 on this quiz? Cognitive psychologists are interested in failures of memory but more broadly target memory organization (its structure) and memory operation (its processes). Of course, the hope is that once we figure these things out, we can answer more specific and individual questions like where your glasses are (have you checked your head?) or how to study effectively (have you tested yourself?). In this chapter we will first describe the structure of long-term memory: We’ll describe different types of long-term memory and their organization. In the next two chapters, we will describe memory processes of encoding (getting memory in) and retrieval (bringing memory back for current use).
A problem can be defined very generally as any situation in which a person has a goal that is not yet accomplished. That definition encompasses what we called decision-making. When psychologists talk about problem-solving, however, they typically mean open-ended problems in which the person knows the goal but nothing in the problem describes how to accomplish the goal.
In Chapter 6 we asked how explicit memory was organized (e.g., how does the concept for “bird” relate to the concept for “robin”), but we only briefly addressed the concepts themselves. This chapter focuses on how we represent categories in the real world by forming mental concepts. As we learn about the world, how do we decide which items belong in the same category? This chapter explores how we draw the lines that define categories and how we use mental representations to do other kinds of thinking.