Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-r7bls Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-14T07:28:09.206Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2013

Robert Kurzban
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104. kurzban@psych.upenn.eduhttps://sites.google.com/site/pleeplab/
Angela Duckworth
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104. duckworth@psych.upenn.eduhttps://sites.sas.upenn.edu/duckworth
Joseph W. Kable
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104. kable@psych.upenn.eduhttp://www.psych.upenn.edu/kable_lab/Joes_Homepage/Home.html
Justus Myers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104. justusm@psych.upenn.edu

Abstract

Why does performing certain tasks cause the aversive experience of mental effort and concomitant deterioration in task performance? One explanation posits a physical resource that is depleted over time. We propose an alternative explanation that centers on mental representations of the costs and benefits associated with task performance. Specifically, certain computational mechanisms, especially those associated with executive function, can be deployed for only a limited number of simultaneous tasks at any given moment. Consequently, the deployment of these computational mechanisms carries an opportunity cost – that is, the next-best use to which these systems might be put. We argue that the phenomenology of effort can be understood as the felt output of these cost/benefit computations. In turn, the subjective experience of effort motivates reduced deployment of these computational mechanisms in the service of the present task. These opportunity cost representations, then, together with other cost/benefit calculations, determine effort expended and, everything else equal, result in performance reductions. In making our case for this position, we review alternative explanations for both the phenomenology of effort associated with these tasks and for performance reductions over time. Likewise, we review the broad range of relevant empirical results from across sub-disciplines, especially psychology and neuroscience. We hope that our proposal will help to build links among the diverse fields that have been addressing similar questions from different perspectives, and we emphasize ways in which alternative models might be empirically distinguished.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, J. M., Goldstein, N. J., Shapiro, J. R. & Bargh, J. A. (2009) You wear me out: The vicarious depletion of self-control. Psychological Science 20(3):326–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, P. L. (2011) 100 years without resting. In: Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications, ed. Ackerman, P. L., pp. 1143. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akerlof, G. A. & Yellen, J. L. (1990) The fair wage–effort hypothesis and unemployment. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 105(2):255–83.Google Scholar
Alexander, G. E. & Crutcher, M. D. (1990) Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: Neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends in Neurosciences 13(7):266–71.Google Scholar
Arai, T. (1912) Mental fatigue. Teachers College, Columbia University, New York.Google Scholar
Aston-Jones, G. & Cohen, J. D. (2005) An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience 28:403–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avena, N. M., Rada, P. & Hoebel, B. G. (2008) Evidence for sugar addiction: Behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32(1):2039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. (1974) Working memory. In: Recent advances in learning and motivation, vol. 8, ed. Bower, G. A., pp. 4790. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Badre, D. & D'Esposito, M. (2009) Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10(9):659–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsh, G. S. & Schwartz, M. W. (2002) Genetic approaches to studying energy balance: Perception and integration. Nature Reviews Genetics 3(8):589600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M. & Tice, D. M. (1998) Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74(5):1252–65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (2007) Self-regulation, ego depletion, and motivation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 1(1):115–28.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D. & Tice, D. M. (2007) The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16(6):351–55.Google Scholar
Beedie, C. J. & Lane, A. M. (2012) The role of glucose in self-control: Another look at the evidence and an alternative conceptualization. Personality and Social Psychology Review 16(2):143–53.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2004) Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiology and Behavior 81(2):179209.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (2007) The debate over dopamine's role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology 191(3):391431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. (2010) How pleasure works: The new science of why we like what we like. W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F. & Lorist, M. M. (2005) Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research 25(1):107–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F. & Lorist, M. M. (2006) Mental fatigue, motivation, and action monitoring. Biological Psychology 72(2):123–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boksem, M. A. S. & Tops, M. (2008) Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews 59(1):125–39. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M. (2007) Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 7(4):356–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M. (2008) Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Science 12(5):201208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108(3):624–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M., Huffstetler, S. & McGuire, J. T. (2009) Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 9(1):1627. doi:10.3758/CABN.9.1.16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M., Jha, A. P., Bylsma, L. M., Fabian, S. A., Solomon, P. E. & Prkachin, K. M. (2005) Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. NeuroImage 25(1):312–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brain, W. R. (1951) Mind, perception, and science. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M. & Cohen, J. D. (1999) Cognition and control in schizophrenia: A computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function. Biological Psychiatry 46(3):312–28.Google Scholar
Buchsbaum, B. R., Greer, S., Chang, W. L. & Berman, K. F. (2005) Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin card-sorting task and component processes. Human Brain Mapping 25(1):3545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnham, T. & Phelan, J. (2000) Mean genes: From sex to money to food: Taming our primal instincts. Perseus.Google Scholar
Bush, G., Vogt, B. A., Holmes, J., Dale, A. M., Greve, D., Jenike, M. A. & Rosen, B. R. (2002) Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A role in reward-based decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99(1):523–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (1989) Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12(1):114.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D. & Semmelroth, J. (1992) Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychological Science 3(4):251–55.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (2000) Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12(1):147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camille, N., Griffiths, C. A., Vo, K., Fellows, L. K. & Kable, J. W. (2011) Ventromedial frontal lobe damage disrupts value maximization in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 31(20):7527–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, E. S., Bridge, M. W. & Jones, D. A. (2009) Carbohydrate sensing in the human mouth: Effects on exercise performance and brain activity. The Journal of Physiology 587(8):1779–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charnov, E. L. (1976) Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology 9(2):129–36.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D., Arps, K., Fultz, J. & Beaman, A. L. (1987) Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52:749–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, D. D. & Sokoloff, L. (1998) Circulation and energy metabolism of the brain. In: Basic neurochemistry: Molecular, cellular, and medical aspects, 6th edition, ed. Siegel, G. J., Agranoff, B. W., Albers, R. W. & Molinoff, P. B., pp. 637–69. Lippincott-Raven.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M. & Yu, A. J. (2007) Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 362(1481):933–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Converse, P. D. & DeShon, R. P. (2009) A tale of two tasks: Reversing the self-regulatory resource depletion effect. Journal of Applied Psychology 94(5):1318–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coull, J. T., Frackowiak, R. S. & Frith, C. D. (1998) Monitoring for target objects: Activation of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task. Neuropsychologia 36(12):1325–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, S. M. (2004) Attention and cognitive control as emergent properties of information representation in working memory. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 4(4):501–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croxson, P. L., Walton, M. E., O'Reilly, J. X., Behrens, T. E. & Rushworth, M. F. (2009) Effort-based cost-benefit valuation and the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience 29(14):4531–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Ardenne, K., McClure, S. M., Nystrom, L. E. & Cohen, J. D. (2008) BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ventral tegmental area. Science 319(5867):1264–67.Google Scholar
D'Esposito, M., Postle, B. R. & Rypma, B. (2000) Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: Evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Experimental Brain Research 133(1):311.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1999) The feelings of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Davies, D. R. & Parasuraman, R. (1982) The psychology of vigilance. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R. & Tsivkin, S. (1999) Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science. 284(5416):970–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derrfuss, J., Brass, M., Neumann, J. & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005) Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Human Brain Mapping 25(1):2234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18:193222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, T. F. & Gailliot, M. T. (2007) Violence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43(1):6276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewitte, S., Bruyneel, S. D. & Geyskens, K. (2009) Self-regulating enhances self-regulation in subsequent consumer decisions involving similar response conflicts. Journal of Consumer Research 36(3):394405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, J. (1997) Why is sex fun? The evolution of human sexuality. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dodge, R. (1917) The laws of relative fatigue. Psychological Review 24(2):89113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, J. (2001) An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2(11):820–29.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, R. (1992) Learned industriousness. Psychological Review 99(2):248–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, R. & Masterson, F. A. (1983) Required high effort increases subsequent persistence and reduces cheating. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44(3):593–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008) Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59:255–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairclough, S. H. & Houston, K. (2004) A metabolic measure of mental effort. Biological Psychology 66(2):177–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A. & Posner, M. I. (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(3):340–47.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., Goette, L. & Zehnder, C. (2009) A behavioral account of the labor market: The role of fairness concerns. Annual Review of Economics 1:355–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fellows, L. K. & Farah, M. J. (2007) The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in decision making: Judgment under uncertainty or judgment per se? Cerebral Cortex 17(11):2669–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fliessbach, K., Weber, B., Trautner, P., Dohmen, T., Sunde, U., Elger, C. E. & Falk, A. (2007) Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum. Science 318(5854):1305–308.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, D. J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T. & Miller, E. K. (2001) Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 291(5502):312–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P. & Miyake, A. (2004) The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General 133(1):101–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Defries, J. C. & Hewitt, J. K. (2006) Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science 17(2):172–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989) Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 61(2):331–49.Google Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1997) Network memory. Trends in Neuroscience 20(10):451–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gailliot, M. T. & Baumeister, R. F. (2007) The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review 11(4):303–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M. & Schmeichel, B. J. (2007) Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92(2):325–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallistel, C. R. (1990) The organization of learning: Learning, development, and conceptual change. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. L. (2007) Carbohydrates and mental function: Feeding or impeding the brain? Nutrition Bulletin 32(Suppl. I):7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. L. & Green, M. W. (2002) Nutritional influences on cognitive function: Mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutrition Research Reviews 15(1):169206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. & Wilson, T. D. (2007) Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317(5843):1351–54.Google Scholar
Glimcher, P. W. (2003) Decisions, uncertainty and the brain: The science of neuroeconomics. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goel, V. (2007) Anatomy of deductive reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Science 11(10):435–41.Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996) Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 93(24):13473–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Muly, E. C. III, & Williams, G. V. (2000) D(1) receptors in prefrontal cells and circuits. Brain Research Reviews 31(2–3):295301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, C., Best, B., Healy, A. F., Kole, J. A. & Bourne, L. E. Jr. (2011) A cognitive modeling account of simultaneous learning and fatigue effects. Cognitive Systems Research 12:1932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopher, D., Brickner, M. & Navon, D. (1982) Different difficulty manipulations interact differently with task emphasis: Evidence for multiple resources. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8:146–57.Google Scholar
Gopher, D. & Navon, D. (1980) How is performance limited: Testing the notion of central capacity. Acta Psychologica 46:161–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grill, H. J. & Kaplan, J. M. (2002) The neuroanatomical axis for control of energy balance. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 23(1):240.Google Scholar
Gunzelmann, G., Gross, J. B., Gluck, K. A. & Dinges, D. F. (2009) Sleep deprivation and sustained attention performance: Integrating mathematical and cognitive modeling. Cognitive Science 33:880910.Google Scholar
Haber, S. N. (2003) The primate basal ganglia: Parallel and integrative networks. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy 26(4):317–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, S. N. & Knutson, B. (2009) The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35(1):426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C. & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2009) The strength model of self-regulation failure and health-related behavior. Health Psychology Review 3(2):208–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C. & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010a) Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 136(4):495525. doi:10.1037/a0019486.Google Scholar
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C. W., Stiff, C. & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010b) Self-regulation and self-control in exercise: The strength-energy model. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 3(1):6286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U. & Burghart, D. R. (2007) Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316(5831):1622–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F. & Rangel, A. (2009) Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324(5927):646–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., Knoepfle, D. T. & Rangel, A. (2010) Value computations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition. Journal of Neuroscience 30(2):583–90.Google Scholar
Hayden, B. Y., Pearson, J. M. & Platt, M. L. (2009) Fictive reward signals in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science 324(5929):948–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Head, H. (1923) The conception of nervous and mental energy (II). Vigilance: A physiological state of the nervous system. British Journal of Psychology 14:126–47.Google Scholar
Healy, A. F., Kole, J. A., Buck-Gengler, C. J. & Bourne, L. E. (2004) Effects of prolonged work on data entry speed and accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 10:188–99.Google Scholar
Hedgcock, W. M., Vohs, K. D. & Rao, A. R. (2012) Reducing self-control depletion effects through enhanced sensitivity to implementation: Evidence from fMRI and behavioral studies. Journal of Consumer Psychology 22(4):486–95.Google Scholar
Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D. & Singer, T. (2010) Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68(1):149–60.Google Scholar
Hickman, K. L., Stromme, C. & Lippman, L. G. (1998) Learned industriousness: Replication in principle. Journal of General Psychology 125(3):213–17.Google Scholar
Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I. & Kramer, A. F. (2008) Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9(1):5865.Google Scholar
Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Castelli, D. M., Hall, E. E. & Kramer, A. F. (2009) The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience 159(3):1044–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockey, G. R. J. (2011) A motivational control theory of cognitive fatigue. In: Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications, ed. Ackerman, P. L., pp. 167–87. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Holding, D. H. (1983) Fatigue. In: Stress and fatigue in human performance, ed. Hockey, G., pp. 145–67. Wiley.Google Scholar
Hommel, J. D., Trinko, R., Sears, R. M., Georgescu, D., Liu, Z., Gao, X. & DiLeone, R. J. (2006) Leptin receptor signaling in midbrain dopamine neurons regulates feeding. Neuron 51(6):801–10.Google Scholar
Huxtable, Z. L., White, M. H. & McCartor, M. A. (1946) A re-performance and re-interpretation of the Arai Experiment in mental fatigue with three subjects. Psychological Monographs 59(5):52.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M. & Gutsell, J. N. (2007) Running on empty: Neural signals for self-control failure. Psychological Science 18(11):933–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, J. M. & Bolstein, R. (1992) Large monetary incentives and their effect on mail survey response rates. Public Opinion Quarterly 56(4):442–53.Google Scholar
Jeukendrup, A. E. & Chambers, E. S. (2010) Oral carbohydrate sensing and exercise performance. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care 13(4):447451.Google Scholar
Job, V., Dweck, C. S. & Walton, G. M. (2010) Ego depletion – Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science 21(11):1686–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A. & Varma, S. (1999) Computational modeling of high-level cognition and brain function. Human Brain Mapping 8:128–36.Google Scholar
Kable, J. W. & Glimcher, P. W. (2007) The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience 10(12):1625–33.Google Scholar
Kable, J. W. & Glimcher, P. W. (2009) The neurobiology of decision: Consensus and controversy. Neuron 63(6):733–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Kanfer, R. (2011) Determinants and consequences of subjective cognitive fatigue. In: Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications, ed. Ackerman, P. L., pp. 189207. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Kennerley, S. W., Dahmubed, A. F., Lara, A. H. & Wallis, J. D. (2009) Neurons in the frontal lobe encode the value of multiple decision variables. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21(6):1162–78.Google Scholar
Kerns, J. G. (2006) Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an fMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task. NeuroImage 33(1):399405.Google Scholar
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A. & Carter, C. S. (2004) Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303(5660):1023–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, L. A. & Ellis, B. J. (2001) An evolutionary-psychological approach to self-esteem: Multiple domains and multiple functions. In: Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Interpersonal processes, ed. Fletcher, G. J. O. & Clark, M. S., pp. 5277. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, L. A., Waugh, C. E., Valencia, A. & Webster, G. D. (2002) The functional domain specificity of self-esteem and the differential prediction of aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82(5):756–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klucharev, V., Hytonen, K., Rijpkema, M., Smidts, A. & Fernandez, G. (2009) Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron 61(1):140–51.Google Scholar
Koechlin, E. & Hyafil, A. (2007) Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making. Science 318(5850):594–98.Google Scholar
Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B. & Botvinick, M. M. (2010) Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 139(4):665–82. doi:10.1037/a0020198.Google Scholar
Krebs, R. M., Boehler, C. N. & Woldorff, M. G. (2010) The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task. Cognition 117(3):341–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhnen, C. M. & Knutson, B. (2005) The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron 47(5):763–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurniawan, I. T., Guitart-Masip, M. & Dolan, R. J. (2011) Dopamine and effort-based decision making. Frontiers in Neuroscience 5:81.Google Scholar
Kurniawan, I. T., Seymour, B., Talmi, D., Yoshida, W., Chater, N. & Dolan, R. J. (2010) Choosing to make an effort: The role of striatum in signaling physical effort of a chosen action. Journal of Neurophysiology 104(1):313–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurzban, R. (2007) Mal's morals. In: The Psychology of Joss Whedon: An unauthorized exploration of Buffy, Angel, and Firefly, ed. Davidson, J. & Wilson, L., pp. 719. [Psychology of Popular Culture Series]. BenBella Books.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2010a) Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks? Evolutionary Psychology 8(2):244–59.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2010b) Why everyone (else) is a hypocrite: Evolution and the modular mind. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2012) Cheatin' hearts and loaded guns: The high fitness stakes of country music lyrics. Review of General Psychology (Special Issue: The Intersection of Evolution and Popular Culture) 16:187–91.Google Scholar
Laird, A. R., McMillan, K. M., Lancaster, J. L., Kochunov, P., Turkeltaub, P. E., Pardo, J. V. & Fox, P. T. (2005) A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Human Brain Mapping 25(1):621.Google Scholar
Laran, J. & Janiszewski, C. (2011) Work or fun? How task construal and completion influence regulatory behavior. Journal of Consumer Research 37(6):967–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lau, B. & Glimcher, P. W. (2008) Value representations in the primate striatum during matching behavior. Neuron 58(3):451–63.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1993) From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing outlooks. Annual Review of Psychology 44(1):122.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R. & Baumeister, R. F. (2000) The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 32:162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K. & Downs, D. L. (1995) Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68(3):518–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, D., Rushworth, M. F., Walton, M. E., Watanabe, M. & Sakagami, M. (2007) Functional specialization of the primate frontal cortex during decision making. Journal of Neuroscience 27(31):8170–73.Google Scholar
Lim, J., Wu, W. C., Wang, J., Detre, J. A., Dinges, D. F. & Rao, H. (2010) Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: An ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. NeuroImage 49(4):3426–35.Google Scholar
Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A. S. & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005) Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research 24(2):199205. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., Jong, R., Mulder, G. & Meijman, T. F. (2000) Mental fatigue and task control: Planning and preparation. Psychophysiology 37(5):614–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynn, M. & McCall, M. (2000) Beyond gratitude and gratuity: A meta-analytic review of the predictors of restaurant tipping. Unpublished manuscript, School of Hotel Administration, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Mackworth, N. H. (1948) The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1:621.Google Scholar
Magen, E. & Gross, J. J. (2007) Harnessing the need for immediate gratification: Cognitive reconstrual modulates the reward value of temptations. Emotion 7(2):415–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Martijn, C., Tenbult, P., Merckelbach, H., Dreezens, E. & de Vries, N. K. (2002) Getting a grip on ourselves: Challenging expectancies about loss of energy after self-control. Social Cognition 20(6):441–60.Google Scholar
Masicampo, E. J. & Baumeister, R. F. (2008) Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological Science 19(3):255–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClure, S. M., Berns, G. S. & Montague, P. R. (2003) Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 38(2):339–46.Google Scholar
McGuire, J. T. & Botvinick, M. M. (2010) Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107(17):7922–26.Google Scholar
Messier, C. (2004) Glucose improvement of memory: A review. European Journal of Pharmacology 490(1–3):3357.Google Scholar
Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24(1):167202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A. (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63(2):8197.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1985) The society of mind. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Mitchell, J. P., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Wyland, C. L., Wegner, D. M. & Neil Macrae, C. (2007) Separating sustained from transient aspects of cognitive control during thought suppression. Psychological Science 18(4):292–97.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A. & Wager, T. D. (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41(1):49100.Google Scholar
Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., Scholer, A. A., Meier, B. P., Noreen, E. E., D'Agostino, P. R. & Martin, V. (2012) Motivational versus metabolic effects of carbohydrates on self-control. Psychological Science 23(10): 1137–44.Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., Dayan, P. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1996) A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. Journal of Neuroscience 16(5):1936–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moray, N. (1967) Where is capacity limited? A survey and a model. Acta Psychologica 27:8492.Google Scholar
Muraven, M. & Baumeister, R. F. (2000) Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin 126(2):247–59. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10748642.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., Shmueli, D. & Burkley, E. (2006) Conserving self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91(3):524–37. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524.Google Scholar
Muraven, M. & Slessareva, E. (2003) Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29(7):894906. doi:10.1177/0146167203029007008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M. & Baumeister, R. F. (1998) Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74(3):774–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naccache, L., Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Habert, M. O., Guichart-Gomez, E., Galanaud, D. & Willer, J. (2005) Effortless control: Executive attention and conscious feeling of mental effort are dissociable. Neuropsychologia 43(9):1318–28.Google Scholar
Nassi, J. J. & Callaway, E. M. (2009) Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10(5):360–72.Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1984) Resources – A theoretical soup stone? Psychological Review 91(2):216–34.Google Scholar
Navon, D. & Gopher, D. (1979) On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review 86(3):214–55.Google Scholar
Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D. & Jonides, J. (2007) Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 7(1):117.Google Scholar
Neumann, J., Lohmann, G., Derrfuss, J. & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005) Meta-analysis of functional imaging data using replicator dynamics. Human Brain Mapping 25(1):165–73.Google Scholar
Newberg, A. B., Wang, J., Rao, H., Swanson, R. L., Wintering, N., Karp, J. S. & Detre, J. A. (2005) Concurrent CBF and CMRG1c changes during human brain activation by combined fMRI-PET scanning. NeuroImage 28(2):500506.Google Scholar
Nieder, A. & Dehaene, S. (2009) Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 32:185208.Google Scholar
Niv, Y., Daw, N. D., Joel, D. & Dayan, P. (2007) Tonic dopamine: Opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191(3):507–20.Google Scholar
Niv, Y., Joel, D. & Dayan, P. (2006) A normative perspective on motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:375–81.Google Scholar
Nix, G. A., Ryan, R. M., Manly, J. B. & Deci, E. L. (1999) Revitalization through self-regulation: The effects of autonomous and controlled motivation on happiness and vitality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35(3):266–84.Google Scholar
O'Connell, K. A., Schwartz, J. E. & Shiffman, S. (2008) Do resisted temptations during smoking cessation deplete or augment self-control resources? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 22(4):486–95.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Friston, K., Critchley, H. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38(2):329–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. (2005) The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(5):242–49.Google Scholar
Orians, G. H. & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992) Evolved responses to landscapes. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 555–79. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orne, M. T. (1962) On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist 17(11):776–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padoa-Schioppa, C. & Assad, J. A. (2006) Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441(7090):223–26.Google Scholar
Padoa-Schioppa, C. & Assad, J. A. (2008) The representation of economic value in the orbitofrontal cortex is invariant for changes of menu. Nature Neuroscience 11(1):95102.Google Scholar
Paus, T., Zatorre, R. J., Hofle, N., Caramanos, Z., Gotman, J., Petrides, M. & Evans, A. C. (1997) Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9(3):392408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Persson, J. & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2010) Training and depletion of executive functions: The case of interference control. Paper presented at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society Annual Meeting, Montreal, Quebec, April 17–20, 2010.Google Scholar
Persson, J., Welsh, K. M., Jonides, J. & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2007) Cognitive fatigue of executive processes: Interaction between interference resolution tasks. Neuropsychologia 45(7):1571–79.Google Scholar
Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (2006) Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442(7106):1042–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, M. (2000) The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory. Experimental Brain Research 133(1):4454.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997) How the mind works. W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Plassmann, H., O'Doherty, J. & Rangel, A. (2007) Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. Journal of Neuroscience 27(37):9984–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975) Attention and cognitive control. In: Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium, ed. Solso, R. L., pp. 5585. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Petersen, S. E. (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 13:2542.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R. & Davidson, B. J. (1980) Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 109:160–74.Google Scholar
Prévost, C., Pessiglione, M., Metereau, E., Clery-Melin, M. L. & Dreher, J. C. (2010) Separate valuation subsystems for delay and effort decision costs. Journal of Neuroscience 30(42):14080–90.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E. & Gusnard, D. A. (2002) Appraising the brain's energy budget. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99(16):10237–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A. & Shulman, G. L. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98(2):676–82.Google Scholar
Rainer, G., Asaad, W. F. & Miller, E. K. (1998) Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature 393(6685):577–79.Google Scholar
Rangel, A., Camerer, C. & Montague, P. R. (2008) A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9(7):545–56. doi:10.1038/nrn2357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rangel, A. & Hare, T. (2010) Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 20(2):262–70.Google Scholar
Rao, S. C., Rainer, G. & Miller, E. K. (1997) Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 276(5313):821–24.Google Scholar
Regan, D. T. (1971) Effects of a favor and liking on compliance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 7(6):627–39.Google Scholar
Robinson, M. D., Schmeichel, B. J. & Inzlicht, M. (2010) A cognitive control perspective of self-control strength and its depletion. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4(3):189200.Google Scholar
Rolfe, J. M. (1971) The secondary task as a measure of mental load. In: Measurement of man at work, ed. Singleton, W. T., Easterby, R. S. & Whitfield, D. E., pp. 135–48. Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Rousselet, G. A., Fabre-Thorpe, M. & Thorpe, S. J. (2002) Parallel processing in high-level categorization of natural images. Nature Neuroscience 5:629–30.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. & Vollmecke, T. A. (1986) Food likes and dislikes. Annual Review of Nutrition 6(1):433–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudebeck, P. H., Behrens, T. E., Kennerley, S. W., Baxter, M. G., Buckley, M. J., Walton, M. E. & Rushworth, F. S. (2008) Frontal cortex subregions play distinct roles in choices between actions and stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience 28(51):13775–85.Google Scholar
Rudebeck, P. H., Walton, M. E., Smyth, A. N., Bannerman, D. M. & Rushworth, M. F. (2006) Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nature Neuroscience 9(9):1161–68.Google Scholar
Rummelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. & The PDP Research Group. (1986) Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Vol. 1. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rutledge, R. B., Lazzaro, S. C., Lau, B., Myers, C. E., Gluck, M. A. & Glimcher, P. W. (2009) Dopaminergic drugs modulate learning rates and perseveration in Parkinson's patients in a dynamic foraging task. Journal of Neuroscience 29(48):15104–14.Google Scholar
Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Farrar, A. M., Nunes, E. J. & Pardo, M. (2009) Dopamine, behavioral economics, and effort. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 3:13.Google Scholar
Samejima, K., Ueda, Y., Doya, K. & Kimura, M. (2005) Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science 310(5752):1337–40.Google Scholar
Scerbo, M. W. (2001) Stress, workload, and boredom in vigilance: A problem and an answer. In: Stress, workload, and fatigue, ed. Hancock, P. A. & Desmond, P. A., pp. 267–78. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scerbo, M. W. & Holcomb, J. R. (1993). Effects of signal conspicuity and time on boredom in vigilance. Paper presented at the First Mid-Atlantic Human Factors Conference, Virginia Beach, Virginia, February 25–26, 1993.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U. (2012) The ironic effect of significant results on the credibility of multiple-study articles. Psychological Methods 17(4):551–66. doi: 10.1037/a0029487.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J. (2007) Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136:241–55.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J. & Vohs, K. (2009) Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96:770–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, F., Gur, R. E., Alavi, A., Seligman, M. E. P., Mozley, L. H., Smith, R. J., Mozley, P. D. & Gur, R. C. (1996) Cerebral blood flow changes in limbic regions induced by unsolvable anagram tasks. American Journal of Psychiatry 153(2):206–12.Google Scholar
Scholey, A. B., Harper, S. & Kennedy, D. O. (2001) Cognitive demand and blood glucose. Physiology and Behavior 73(4):585–92.Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275(5306):1593–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M. & Dehaene, S. (2008) Brain mechanisms of serial and parallel processing during dual-task performance. The Journal of Neuroscience 28(30):7585–98.Google Scholar
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (2004) Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303(5661):1157–62.Google Scholar
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (2006) Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439(7075):466–69.Google Scholar
Singh, D. (1993) Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65(2):293307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E. & Jonides, J. (1998) Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95(20):12061–68.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994) The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In: Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, ed. Hirschfeld, L. & Gelman, S. A., pp. 3967. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stucke, T. S. & Baumeister, R. F. (2006) Ego depletion and aggressive behavior: Is the inhibition of aggression a limited resource? European Journal of Social Psychology 36(1):113.Google Scholar
Stuss, D. T. & Alexander, M. P. (2000) Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research 63(3):289–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A. G. (1998) Reinforcement learning: An introduction. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Symons, D. (1992) On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 137–59. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Bedny, M. & Goldberg, R. F. (2005) The frontal lobes and the regulation of mental activity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15(2):219–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorndike, E. L. (1904) An introduction to the theory of mental and social measurements. Science Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R. (1998) Darwinian aesthetics. In: Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications, ed. Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. L., pp. 543–72. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D. & Muraven, M. (2007) Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43(3):379–84.Google Scholar
Tom, S. M., Fox, C. R., Trepel, C. & Poldrack, R. A. (2007) The neural basis of loss aversion in decision-making under risk. Science 315(5811):515–18.Google Scholar
Tomporowski, P. D. (2003) Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychologica 112(3):297324.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 19136 . Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., Sell, A., Lieberman, D. & Sznycer, D. (2008) Internal regulatory variables and the design of human motivation: A computational and evolutionary approach. In: Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation, ed. Elliot, A., pp. 251–71. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tops, M., Lorist, M. M., Wijers, A. A. & Meijman, T. F. (2004) To stress or relax: Neurochemical aspects of activity and rest. Gedrag en Organisatie 17:3242.Google Scholar
Tyler, J. M. & Burns, K. C. (2008) After depletion: The replenishment of the self's regulatory resources. Self and Identity 7:305–21.Google Scholar
van der Linden, D. (2011) The urge to stop: The cognitive and biological nature of acute mental fatigue. In: Cognitive fatigue: Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications, ed. Ackerman, P. L., pp. 149–64. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Linden, D., Frese, M. & Meijman, T. F. (2003) Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychologica 113(1):4565. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00150-6.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M. & Tice, D. M. (2008) Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94(5):883–98.Google Scholar
Wager, T. D. & Smith, E. E. (2003) Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 3(4):255–74.Google Scholar
Walton, M. E., Bannerman, D. M., Alterescu, K.. & Rushworth, M. F. (2003) Functional specialization within medial frontal cortex of the anterior cingulate for evaluating effort-related decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience 23(16):6475–79.Google Scholar
Warm, J. S. (1984) An introduction to vigilance. In: Sustained attention in human performance, ed. Warm, J. S., pp. 114. Wiley.Google Scholar
Warm, J. S., Matthews, G. & Finomore, V. S. Jr. (2008) Vigilance, workload, and stress. In: Performance under stress, ed. Hancock, P. A. & Szalma, J., pp. 115–41. Ashgate.Google Scholar
Webb, T. L. & Sheeran, P. (2003) Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego-depletion? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39(3):279–86.Google Scholar
Wickens, C. D. (2002) Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science 3(2):159–77.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wright, R. A., Junious, T. R., Neal, C., Avello, A., Graham, C., Herrmann, L., Junious, S. & Walton, N. (2007) Mental fatigue on effort-related cardiovascular response: Difficulty effects and extension across cognitive performance domains. Motivation and Emotion 31:219–31.Google Scholar
Wyland, C. L., Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Gordon, H. L. & Heatherton, T. F. (2003) Neural correlates of thought suppression. Neuropsychologia 41(14):1863–67.Google Scholar
Zaghloul, K. A., Blanco, J. A., Weidemann, C. T., McGill, K., Jaggi, J. L., Baltuch, G. H. & Kahana, M. J. (2009) Human substantia nigra neurons encode unexpected financial rewards. Science 323(5920):1496–99.Google Scholar