Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

Forward models and their implications for production, comprehension, and dialogue

  • Martin J. Pickering (a1) and Simon Garrod (a2)
Abstract

Our target article proposed that language production and comprehension are interwoven, with speakers making predictions of their own utterances and comprehenders making predictions of other people's utterances at different linguistic levels. Here, we respond to comments about such issues as cognitive architecture and its neural basis, learning and development, monitoring, the nature of forward models, communicative intentions, and dialogue.

Copyright
References
Hide All
Adank, P., Hagoort, P. & Bekkering, H. (2010) Imitation improves language comprehension. Psychological Science 21:1903–909.
Barsalou, L. (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:577600.
D'Ausilio, A., Jarmolowska, J., Busan, P., Bufalari, I. & Craighero, L. (2011) Tongue corticospinal modulation during attended verbal stimuli: Priming and coarticulation effects. Neuropsychologia 49:3670–76.
Dahan, D., Drucker, S. J. & Scarborough, R. A. (2008) Talker adaptation in speech perception: Adjusting the signal or the representations? Cognition 108:710–18.
De Ruiter, J. P., Mitterer, H. & Enfield, N. J. (2006) Predicting the end of a speaker's turn; a cognitive cornerstone of conversation. Language 82(3):515–35.
Dell, G. S. (1986) A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review 93:283321.
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P. & Kutas, M. (2005) Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience 8(8):1117–21.
Flinker, A., Chang, E. F., Kirsch, H. E., Barbaro, N. M., Crone, N. E. & Knight, R.T. (2010) Single-trial speech suppression of auditory cortex activity in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 30:16643–50.
Glenberg, A. M. & Gallese, V. (2012) Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex 48(7):905–22. DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010.
Grossberg, S. (1980) How does a brain build a cognitive code? Psychological Review 87(1):151.
Heinks-Maldonado, T. H., Nagarajan, S. S. & Houde, J. F. (2006) Magnetoencephalographic evidence for a precise forward model in speech production. NeuroReport 17(13):1375–79.
Huettig, F. & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2010) Listening to yourself is like listening to others: External, but not internal, verbal self-monitoring is based on speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes 25:347–74.
Hurley, S. (2008a) The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(01):122.
Jordan, M. I. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1992) Forward models: Supervised learning with a distal teacher. Cognitive Science, 16: 307–54.
Kawato, M., Furawaka, K. & Suzuki, R. (1987) A hierarchical neural network model for the control and learning of voluntary movements. Biological Cybernetics 56: 117.
Kawato, M., Maeda, Y., Uno, Y. & Suzuki, R. (1990) Trajectory formation of arm movement by cascade neural network model based on minimum torque-change criterion. Biological Cybernetics, 62: 275–88.
Lesage, E., Morgan, B. E., Olson, A. C., Meyer, A. S. & Miall, R. C. (2012) Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Current Biology 22, R794–95.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.
Mani, N. & Huettig, F. (2012) Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake – but only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 38: 843–47.
Miall, R. C. & Wolpert, D. M. (1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks 9: 1265–79.
Nozari, N., Dell, G. S. & Schwartz, M. F. (2011) Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. Cognitive Psychology 63(1):133. DOI:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.05.001.
Ondobaka, S., de Lange, F. P., Newman-Norlund, R. D., Wiemers, M. & Bekkering, H. (2011) Interplay between action and movement intentions during social interaction. Psychological Science 23: 3035.
Pezzulo, G. (2011a) Grounding procedural and declarative knowledge in sensorimotor anticipation. Mind and Language 26:78114.
Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(2):169226.
Tremblay, S., Shiller, D. M. & Ostry, D. J. (2003) Somatosensory basis of speech production. Nature 423: 866–69.
Trude, A. M. & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012) Talker-specific perceptual adaptation during on-line speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes 27: 9791001.
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Flanagan, J. R. (2001) Perspectives and problems in motor learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:487–94.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed