Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T08:14:14.763Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

If priming is graded rather than all-or-none, can reactivating abstract structures be the underlying mechanism?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2017

Laurie Beth Feldman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Albany, Albany, NY 12222. lfeldman@albany.edu
Petar Milin
Affiliation:
Department of Journalism Studies, The University of Sheffield, Sheffield S1 4DT, United Kingdom. p.milin@sheffield.ac.uk

Abstract

In our commentary on Branigan & Pickering (B&P), we start by arguing that the authors implicitly adopt several assumptions, the consequence of which is to make further claims necessary and/or sufficient. Crucially, the authors assume the existence of discrete units at various levels of linguistic granularity that then must be operated upon by combinatorial mechanisms and rules (i.e., decomposition/recomposition). They further argue that structural priming provides a powerful tool to study abstract, structural representations. We provide evidence that priming effects in production are characterized better as graded than as all-or-none and that priming need not arise from a mechanism that (re)activates a shared but abstract internal structure.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baayen, R. H., Milin, P. & Ramscar, M. (2016a) Frequency in lexical processing. Aphasiology 30(11):1174–220.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Shaoul, C., Willits, J. & Ramscar, M. (2016b) Comprehension without segmentation: A proof of concept with naive discrimination learning. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience. 31(1):106–28Google Scholar
Cleland, A. A. & Pickering, M. J. (2003) The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language 49(2):214–30. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00060-3.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Milin, P. & Baayen, R. H. (2013a) Frequency and regularity effects do not interact in verb production. Presentation at European Society for Cognitive Psychology. Marseilles, France. August 30, 2013.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Milin, P. & Baayen, R. H. (2013b) Frequency and regularity effects do not interact in verb production: A challenge to dual mechanism accounts of inflectional processing. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing, Budapest, Hungary. August 22, 2013.Google Scholar
Milin, P., Feldman, L. B., Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P. & Baayen, R. H. (2017) Discrimination in lexical decision. PLoS ONE 12(2):e0171935. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171935.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Branigan, H. P. (1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 39(4):633–51.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Dye, M. & McCauley, S. M. (2013) Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of mouses in adult speech. Language 89(4):760–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar