Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Resource-rationality beyond individual minds: the case of interactive language use

  • Mark Dingemanse (a1) (a2) (a3)

Abstract

Resource-rational approaches offer much promise for understanding human cognition, especially if they can reach beyond the confines of individual minds. Language allows people to transcend individual resource limitations by augmenting computation and enabling distributed cognition. Interactive language use, an environment where social rational agents routinely deal with resource constraints together, offers a natural laboratory to test resource-rationality in the wild.

Copyright

References

Hide All
Bender, A. & Beller, S. (2014) Mangarevan invention of binary steps for easier calculation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111(4):1322–27. doi:10.1073/pnas.1309160110.
Böckler, A., Knoblich, G. & Sebanz, N. (2010) Socializing cognition. In: Towards a theory of thinking, ed. Glatzeder, B., Goel, V. & Müller, A., pp. 233–50. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-03129-8_16. Heidelberg.
Chomsky, N. (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1998) Magic words: How language augments human computation. In: Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes, ed. Carruthers, P. & Boucher, J., pp. 162–83. Cambridge University Press.
Clark, A. (2006) Material symbols. Philosophical Psychology 19(3):291307. doi:10.1080/09515080600689872.
Dingemanse, M. (2017) On the margins of language: Ideophones, interjections and dependencies in linguistic theory. In: Dependencies in language, ed. Enfield, N. J., pp. 195202. doi:10.5281/zenodo.573781. Language Science Press.
Dingemanse, M., Roberts, S. G., Baranova, J., Blythe, J., Drew, P., Floyd, S., Gisladottir, R. S., Kendrick, K. H., Levinson, S. C., Manrique, E., Rossi, G. & Enfield, N. J. (2015) Universal principles in the repair of communication problems. PLoS One 10(9):e0136100. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0136100.
Enfield, N. J. (2013) Relationship thinking: Agency, enchrony, and human sociality. Oxford University Press.
Enfield, N. J. (2017) How we talk: The inner workings of conversation. Basic Books.
Fox Tree, J. E. (1995) The effects of false starts and repetitions on the processing of subsequent words in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language 34(6):709–38. doi:10.1006/jmla.1995.1032.
Fox Tree, J. E. (2001) Listeners’ uses of um anduh in speech comprehension. Memory & Cognition 29(2):320–26. doi:10.3758/BF03194926.
Frank, M. C. & Goodman, N. D. (2012) Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science 336(6084):998. doi:10.1126/science.1218633.
Fusaroli, R., Tylén, K., Garly, K., Steensig, J., Christiansen, M. H. & Dingemanse, M. (2017) Measures and mechanisms of common ground: Backchannels, conversational repair, and interactive alignment in free and task-oriented social interactions. In: Proceedings of the 39th annual meeting of the cognitive science society, ed. Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T. & Davelaar, E., pp. 2055–60.
Goffman, E. (1967) Interaction ritual. Aldine.
Heyes, C. (2018) Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Harvard University Press.
Hirsch, J., Adam Noah, J., Zhang, X., Dravida, S. & Ono, Y. (2018) A cross-brain neural mechanism for human-to-human verbal communication. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 13(9):907–20. doi:10.1093/scan/nsy070.
Hutchins, E. (1995) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press.
Kempson, R., Cann, R., Gregoromichelaki, E. & Chatzikyriakidis, S. (2016) Language as mechanisms for interaction. Theoretical Linguistics 42(3–4):203–76. doi:10.1515/tl-2016-0011.
Konvalinka, I. & Roepstorff, A. (2012) The two-brain approach: How can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:215. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00215.
Lestrade, S. (2017) Unzipping Zipf's law. PLoS One 12(8):e0181987. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0181987.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.
Levinson, S. C. (2000) Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. MIT Press.
Levinson, S. C. (2016) Turn-taking in human communication – origins and implications for language processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(1):614. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010.
Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H. & Gibson, E. (2012) The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition 122(3):280–91. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.004.
Risko, E. F. & Gilbert, S. J. (2016) Cognitive offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(9):676–88. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.002.
Roberts, S. G. & Levinson, S. C. (2017) Conversation, cognition and cultural evolution: A model of the cultural evolution of word order through pressures imposed from turn taking in conversation. Interaction Studies 18(3):404–31. doi:10.1075/is.18.3.06rob
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T. & Vogeley, K. (2013) Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4):393414. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000660.
Van Rooij, I., Kwisthout, J., Blokpoel, M., Szymanik, J., Wareham, T. & Toni, I. (2011) Intentional communication: Computationally easy or difficult? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5:118. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00052
Waldron, V. R. & Cegala, D. J. (1992) Assessing conversational cognition: Levels of cognitive theory and associated methodological requirements. Human Communication Research 18(4):599622. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1992.tb00573.x.
Zipf, G. K. (1949) Human behavior and the principle of least effort: An introduction to human ecology. Addison-Wesley Press.

Resource-rationality beyond individual minds: the case of interactive language use

  • Mark Dingemanse (a1) (a2) (a3)

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed