Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T11:36:50.728Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Joan Bybee
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersen, Henning. 1973. Abductive and deductive change. Language 49: 765–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1982. Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review 89: 369–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1993. Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, John R. and Bower, Gordon H.. 1973. A propositional theory of recognition memory. Memory and Cognition 2(3): 406–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, Michael A. 2003. The evolving mirror system: a neural basis for language readiness. In Christiansen, Morten H. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language evolution, 182–200. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Aske, Jon. 1990. Disembodied rules vs. patterns in the lexicon: testing the psychological reality of Spanish stress rules. Berkeley Linguistics Society 16: 30–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, Harald. 1993. On frequency, transparency, and productivity. In Booij, G. E. and Marle, J. (eds.), Yearbook of morphology, 181–208. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Baayen, Harald. 2003. Probabilistic approaches to morphology. In Bod, R., Hay, J. and Jannedy, S. (eds.), Probability theory in linguistics, 229–87. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bailey, Todd M., and Hahn, Ulrike. 2001. Determinants of wordlikeness: phonotactics or lexical neighborhoods? Journal of Memory and Language 44: 568–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, Naomi. 1977. Language acquisition and historical change. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth. 1994. Modularity, domain specificity and the development of language. In Gajdusek, D.C., McKhann, G.M., and Bolis, C.L. (eds.), Evolution and the neurology of language. Discussions in neuroscience 10(1–2): 136–49.
Bates, E., Bertherton, I. and Snyder, L.. 1988. From first words to grammar: individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, Thal, Donna and Marchman, Virginia. 1991. Symbols and syntax: a Darwinian approach to language development. In Krasnegor, N., Rumbaugh, D. M., Schiefelbusch, R. L. and Kennedy, M. Studdert- (eds.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development, 29–65. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Beckner, Clay, and Bybee, Joan. 2009. A usage-based account of constituency and reanalysis. Language Learning 59: Suppl. 1, December, 27–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, Alan, Jurafsky, Daniel, Fosler-Lussier, Eric, Girand, Cynthia, Gregory, Michelle and Gildea, Daniel. 2003. Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 113(2): 1001–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, Brent and Kay, Paul. 1969. Basic color terms: their universality and evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, Basel. 1972. Social class, language and socialization. In Giglioli, P. (ed.), Language and social context. Baltimore, MD: Penguin.Google Scholar
Beths, Frank. 1999. The history of DARE and the status of unidirectionality. Linguistics 37: 1069–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biber, Douglas. 1986. Spoken and written textual dimensions in English. Language 62: 384–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 1981. Roots of language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.Google Scholar
Binnick, Robert I. 1991. Time and the verb. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bisang, Walter. 2004. Grammaticalization without coevolution of form and meaning: the case of tense-aspect-modality in East and mainland Southeast Asia. In Bisang, W., Himmelmann, N., and Wiemer, B. (eds.), What makes grammaticalization? A look from its fringes and its components, 109–38. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, Hans. 2003. A constructional approach to resultatives. (Stanford Monographs in Linguistics). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Bowdle, Brian F. and Gentner, Dedre. 2005. The career of metaphor. Psychological Review 112: 193–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyland, Joyce T. 1996. Morphosyntactic change in progress: a psycholinguistic approach. Dissertation. University of California, Berkeley, CA.Google Scholar
Bradley, H. 1904. The making of English. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., and Franks, J. J.. 1971. The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology 2: 331–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinton, Laurel and Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2005. Lexicalization and language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briscoe, Ted. 2003. Grammatical assimilation. In Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language evolution, 295–316. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Browman, Catherine P., and Goldstein, Louis M.. 1992. Articulatory phonology: an overview. Phonetica 49: 155–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, Esther. 2004. Reduction of syllable initial /s/ in the Spanish of New Mexico and southern Colorado: a usage based approach. Dissertation. University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1985. Morphology: a study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1986. On the nature of grammatical categories: a diachronic perspective. In Choi, S. (ed.), Proceedings of the Second Eastern States Conference on Linguistics, 17–34.
Bybee, Joan L. 1988a. Morphology as lexical organization. In Hammond, M. and Noonan, M. (eds.), Theoretical morphology, 119–41. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1988b. Semantic substance vs. contrast in the development of grammatical meaning. Berkeley Linguistics Society 14: 247–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1988c. The diachronic dimension in explanation. In Hawkins, J. (ed.), Explaining language universals, 350–79. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1994. The grammaticization of zero: asymmetries in tense and aspect systems. In Pagliuca, W. (ed.), Perspectives on grammaticalization, 235–54. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1995. The semantic development of past tense modals in English. In Bybee, J. and Fleischman, S. (eds.) Modality in grammar and discourse, 503–7. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1997. Semantic aspects of morphological typology. In Bybee, J., Haiman, J. and Thompson, S. (eds.), Essays on language function and language type, 25–37. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1998a. A functionalist approach to grammar and its evolution. Evolution of Communication 2: 249–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1998b. ‘Irrealis’ as a grammatical category. Anthropological linguistics 40: 257–71.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 1998c. The emergent lexicon. CLS 34: the panels, 421–35. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 279–93.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2000a. Lexicalization of sound change and alternating environments. In Broe, M. and Pierrehumbert, J. (eds.), Laboratory Phonology 5: Language acquisition and the lexicon, 250–68. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 216–34.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2000b. The phonology of the lexicon: evidence from lexical diffusion. In Barlow, M. and Kemmer, S. (eds.), Usage-based models of language, 65–85. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 199–215.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2001a. Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2001b. Main clauses are innovative, subordinate clauses are conservative: consequences for the nature of constructions. In Bybee, J. and Noonan, M. (eds.), Complex sentences in grammar and discourse: essays in honor of Sandra A. Thompson, 1–17. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2002a. Sequentiality as the basis of constituent structure. In Givón, T. and Malle, B. (eds.), The evolution of language from pre-language, 109–32. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 313–35.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2002b. Word frequency and context use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically conditioned sound change. Language Variation and Change 14: 261–90. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 235–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2003a. Cognitive processes in grammaticalization. In Tomasello, M. (ed.), The new psychology of language, Vol. II, 145–67. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2003b. Mechanisms of change in grammaticization: the role of frequency. In Joseph, B. D. and Janda, R. D. (eds.), The handbook of historical linguistics, 602–23. Oxford: Blackwell. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 336–57.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2005. Restrictions on phonemes in affixes: a crosslinguistic test of a popular hypothesis. Linguistic Typology 9: 165–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2006a. From usage to grammar: the mind's response to repetition. Language 82: 711–733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2006b. Language change and universals. In Mairal, R. and Gil, J. (eds.), Linguistic universals, 179–94. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2007. Frequency of use and the organization of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2008. Formal universals as emergent phenomena: the origins of Structure Preservation. In Good, J. (ed.), Language universals and language change, 108–21. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2009a. Grammaticization: implications for a theory of language. In Guo, J., Lieven, E., Ervin-Tripp, S., Budwig, N., Őzçalişkan, S., and Nakamura, K. (eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin, 345–56. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2009b. Language universals and usage-based theory. In Christiansen, M. H., Collins, C., and Edelman, S. (eds.), Language universals, 17–39. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Brewer, Mary A.. 1980. Explanation in morphophonemics: changes in Provençal and Spanish preterite forms. Lingua 52: 201–42. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 41–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Pardo, Elly. 1981. On lexical and morphological conditioning of alternations: a nonce-probe experiment with Spanish verbs. Linguistics 19: 937–68. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 74–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Slobin, Dan I.. 1982. Why small children cannot change language on their own: evidence from the English past tense. In Alqvist, A. (ed.), Papers from the 5th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, 29–37. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Moder, Carol L.. 1983. Morphological classes as natural categories. Language 59: 251–70. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 127–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Pagliuca, William. 1987. The evolution of future meaning. In Ramat, A. Giacalone, Carruba, O. and Bernini, G. (eds.), Papers from the 7th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, 109–22. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Dahl, Östen. 1989. The creation of tense and aspect systems in the languages of the world. Studies in language 13(1): 51–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., Pagliuca, William, and Perkins, Revere. 1991. Back to the future. In Traugott, E. and Heine, B. (eds.), Approaches to grammaticalization, Vol.II, 17–58. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan, Perkins, Revere, and Pagliuca, William. 1994. The evolution of grammar: tense, aspect and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan, and Scheibman, Joanne. 1999. The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: the reduction of don't in English. Linguistics 37(4): 575–96. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 294–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan, and Thompson, Sandra A.. 2000. Three frequency effects in syntax. Berkeley Linguistics Society 23: 65–85. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 269–78.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan, and McClelland, James L.. 2005. Alternatives to the combinatorial paradigm of linguistic theory based on domain general principles of human cognition. In Ritter, N. A. (ed.), The role of linguistics in cognitive science. Special issue of The Linguistic Review, 22(2–4): 381–410.
Bybee, Joan, and Eddington, David. 2006. A usage-based approach to Spanish verbs of ‘becoming’. Language 82: 323–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan, and Cacoullos, Rena Torres. 2009. The role of prefabs in grammaticization: How the particular and the general interact in language change. In R. Corrigan, E. Moravcsik, H. Ouali, and K. Wheatley (eds.), Formulaic language, Vol. I, 187–217. Typological Studies in Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Campbell, Alistair. 1959. Old English grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, Lyle. 2001. What's wrong with grammaticalization? In Campbell, L. (ed.), Grammaticalization: a critical assessment. Special issue of Language Sciences23 (2–3), 113–61.
Carey, Kathleen. 1994. The grammaticalization of the Perfect in Old English: an account based on pragmatics and metaphor. In Pagliuca, W. (ed.), Perspectives on grammaticalization, 103–17. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Casenheiser, Devin, and Goldberg, Adele E.. 2005. Fast mapping of a phrasal form and meaning. Developmental Science 8: 500–08.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace. 1982. Integration and involvement in speaking, writing and oral literature. In Tanner, D. (ed.), Spoken and written language: exploring orality and literacy, 35–53. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Chevrot, Jean-Pierre, Beaud, Laurence and Varga, Renata. 2000. Developmental data on a French sociolinguistic variable: post-consonantal word-final /R/. Language Variation and Change 12: 295–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2006. On phases. In Freidin, R., Otero, C. and Zubizaretta, M. (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory, 133–66. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clausner, Tim, and Croft, William. 1997. The productivity and schematicity of metaphor. Cognitive Science 21: 247–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coates, Jennifer. 1983. The semantics of the modal auxiliary. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Coleman, John, and Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1997. Stochastic phonological grammars and acceptability. Computational phonology: proceedings of the 3rd meeting of the ACL Special Interest Group in Computational Phonology, 49–56. Somerset: Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Company Company, Concepción . 2006. Subjectification of verbs into discourse markers: semantic-pragmatic change only? In Cornillie, B. and Delbecque, N. (eds.), Topics in subjectification and modalization, 97–121. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Comrie, Bernard. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Comrie, Bernard. 1985. Tense. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Contini-Morava, Ellen. 1989. Discourse pragmatics and semantic categorization: the case of negation and tense-aspect with special reference to Swahili. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Coste, Jean, and Redondo, Augustin. 1965. Syntaxe de l'espagnol moderne. Paris: Société d'Edition d'Enseignement Superieur.Google Scholar
Croft, William. 2000. Explaining language change. Harlow: Longman Linguistic Library.Google Scholar
Croft, William. 2001. Radical construction grammar: syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William. 2003. Typology and universals (2nd edn). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Croft, William, and Cruse, Alan. 2004. Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. 1999. Syntactic nuts: hard cases, syntactic theory, and language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter W., and Jackendoff, Ray. 2005. Simpler syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curme, George O. 1931. A grammar of the English language. Essex: Verbatim.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, Eva, and Lieven, Elena. 2005. Towards a lexically specific grammar of children's question constructions. Cognitive Linguistics 16: 437–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahl, Östen. 1985. Tense and aspect systems. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Dahl, Östen. 1995. The marking of the episodic/generic distinction in tense-aspect systems. In Carlson, G. and Pelletier, F. (eds.), The generic book, 412–25. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Dahl, Östen. 2001. Inflationary effects in language and elsewhere. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 471–80. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dahl, Östen. 2006. Corpus del español. (100 million words, 1200s–1900s). Available online at www.corpusdelespanol.org. Accessed Autumn 2006.
Denison, David. 1985. The origins of periphrastic ‘do’: Ellegård and Visser reconsidered. In Eaton, R., Fischer, O., Leek, F., and Koopman, W. F. (eds.), In Papers from the 4th International Conference on English Historical Linguistics, 45–60. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Denison, David. 1993. English historical syntax: verbal constructions. London: Longmans.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, Manuel. 2004. Acquisition of sociolinguistic variables in Spanish: do children acquire individual lexical forms or variable rules? In Face, T. (ed.), Laboratory approaches to Spanish phonology, 221–36. Berlin: DeGruyter.Google Scholar
D' Introno, Franco, and Sosa, Juan Manuel.. 1986. Elisión de la /d/ en el español de Caracas: aspectos sociolingüísticos e implicaciones teóricas. In Cedeño, R. A. Núñez, Urdaneta, I. Páez, and Guitart, J. (eds.), Estudios sobre la fonología del español del Caribe, 135–63. Caracas: Ediciones La Casa de Bello.Google Scholar
Diessel, Holger, and Tomasello, Michael. 2005. A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses. Language 81: 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, R., Klein, Wolfgang and Noyau, C.. 1995. The acquisition of temporality in a second language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diver, William. 1964. The system of agency in the Latin noun. Word 20: 178–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. 1972. The Dyirbal language of North Queensland. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobzhansky, Theodosius. 1964. Biology, molecular and organismic. American Zoologist 4: 443–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donald, Merlin. 1991. Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, Merlin. 1998. Mimesis and the executive suite: missing links in language evolution. In Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Knight, C. (eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language, 44–67. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Downing, Pamela. 1977. On the creation and use of English compound nouns. Language 53: 810–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drachman, Gaberell. 1978. Child language and language change: a conjecture and some refutations. In Fisiak, J. (ed.), Recent developments in historical phonology, 123–44. Berlin: Walter De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dryer, Matthew S. 1988. Object-verb order and adjective-noun order: dispelling a myth. Lingua 74: 185–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Du Bois, John W. 1985. Competing motivations. In Haiman, J. (ed.), Iconicity in syntax, 343–65. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Du Bois, John W. 1987. The discourse basis of ergativity. Language 63: 805–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eddington, David. 1999. On ‘becoming’ in Spanish: a corpus analysis of verbs of expressing change of state. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 18: 23–46.Google Scholar
Eddington, David. 2000. Stress assignment in Spanish within the analogical modeling of language. Language 76: 92–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellegård, Alvar. 1953. The auxiliary DO: the establishment and regulation of its use in English. Stockholm: Almqvist and Wiksell.Google Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 1996. Sequencing in SLA: phonological memory, chunking and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18: 91–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Larsen-Freeman, Diane. 2006. Language emergence: implications for applied linguistics – introduction to the special issue. Applied linguistics 27(4): 558–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erman, Britt, and Warren, Beatrice. 2000. The Idiom Principle and the Open Choice Principle. Text 20: 29–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fente, R. 1970. Sobre los verbos de cambio o devenir. Filología Moderna 38: 157–72.Google Scholar
Fidelholtz, James. 1975. Word frequency and vowel reduction in English. Chicago Linguistic Society 11: 200–13.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J., and Kay, Paul. 1999. Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: the What's X doing Y? construction. Language 75(1): 1–33.CrossRef
Fillmore, Charles J., Kay, Paul and O' Connor, Mary C.. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions. Language 64: 501–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga. 2007. Morphosyntactic change. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Gerald, and Docherty, Paul. 2006. The social life of phonetics and phonology. Journal of Phonetics 34: 409–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Carol A., and Housum, Jonathan. 1987. Talkers' signaling of “new” and “old” words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language 26: 489–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Barbara A. 1987. The Noun Phrase Accessibility Hierarchy reinterpreted: subject primacy or the absolutive hypothesis. Language 63: 856–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, Stefan A., Large, Nathan R., Zawaydeh, Bushra and Pisoni, David B.. 2001. Emergent phonotactic generalizations in English and Arabic. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 159–80. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
García, Erica., and Putte, Florimon. 1989. Forms are silver, nothing is gold. Folia Linguistica Historica 8(1–2): 365–84.Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre. 1983. Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive science 7: 155–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, and Markman, Arthur B.. 1997. Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist 52: 45–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W., and O' Brien, Jennifer E.. 1990. Idioms and mental imagery: the metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition 36(11): 35–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Givón, Talmy. 1971. Historical syntax and synchronic morphology: an archeologist's field trip. CLS 7: 384–415. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy. 1973. The time-axis phenomenon. Language 49: 890–925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy. 1975. Serial verbs and syntactic change: Niger-Congo. In Charles N. Li (ed.), Word order and word order change, 47–112. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
Givón, Talmy. 1979. On understanding grammar. New York/San Francisco: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy. 1984. Syntax: a functional-typological introduction, Vol. I. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 2002. Biolinguistics: the Santa Barbara lectures. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: a construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2003. Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 219–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E., Casenheiser, Devin and Sethuraman, N.. 2004. Learning argument structure generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics 14: 289–316.Google Scholar
Goldinger, Stephen. 1996. Word and voices: episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology 22: 1166–83.Google Scholar
Goldinger, Stephen, Luce, Paul and Pisoni, David. 1989. Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language 28: 501–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, John, and Woisetschlaeger, E.. 1982. The logic of the English progressive. Linguistic Inquiry 13: 79–89.Google Scholar
Goossens, Louis. 1987. The auxiliarization of the English modals: a functional grammar view. In Harris, M. and Ramat, P. (eds.), Historical development of auxiliaries (Trends in linguistics, 35), 111–43. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Goossens, Louis. 1990. Cunnan, conne(n), can: the development of a radial category. In Kellermann, G. and Morrissey, M. D. (eds.), Diachrony within synchrony: language history and cognition, 377–94. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1963. Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. Greenberg (ed.), Universals of language, 73–113. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1966. Language universals: with special reference to feature hierarchies. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1969. Some methods of dynamic comparison in linguistics. In Puhvel, J. (ed.), Substance and structure of language, 147–203. Berkeley/Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1978a. Diachrony, synchrony and language universals. In Greenberg, J., Ferguson, C. and Moravcsik, E. (eds.), Universals of human language: method and theory, Vol.I, 61–92. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1978b. How do languages acquire gender markers? In Greenberg, J., Ferguson, C. and Moravcsik, E. (eds.), Universals of human language, Vol.III, 47–82. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H., Ferguson, Charles and Moravcsik, Edith (eds.). 1978. Universals of human language: method and theory. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Gregory, Michelle, Raymond, William, Bell, Alan, Fosler-Lussier, Eric and Jurafsky, Daniel. 1999. The effects of collocational strength and contextual predictability in lexical production. CLS35: 151–66. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Gries, Stefan, Hampe, Beate and Schönefeld, Doris. 2005. Converging evidence: bringing together experimental and corpus data on the association of verbs and constructions. Cognitive Linguistics 16(4): 635–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurevich, Olga, Johnson, Matt, and Goldberg, Adele E.. To appear. Incidental verbatim memory for language. Language and Cognition.
Haiman, John. 1985. Natural syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Haiman, John. 1994. Ritualization and the development of language. In Pagliuca, W. (ed.), Perspectives on grammaticalization, 3–28. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Haiman, John. 2002. Systematization and the origin of rules: the case of subject-verb inversion in questions. Studies in Language 26(3): 573–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halle, Morris. 1962. Phonology in generative grammar. Word 18: 54–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan. 2006. An acoustic analysis of ‘happy-tensing’ in the Queen's Christmas broadcasts. Journal of Phonetics 34: 439–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Alice C., and Campbell, Lyle. 1995. Historical syntax in cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1989. From purposive to infinitive – a universal path of grammaticization, Folia Linguistica Historica 10: 287–310.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1998. Does grammaticalization need reanalysis? Studies in Language 22(2): 315–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, Anna G. 1951. The use of the Progressive form in English. Language 27: 254–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 1983. Word order universals. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 1994. A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2004. Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2009. Language universals and the Performance-Grammar Correspondence Hypothesis. In Christiansen, M., Collins, C. and Edelman, S. (eds.), Language universals, 54–78. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer. 2001. Lexical frequency in morphology: is everything relative?Linguistics 39: 1041–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer. 2002. From speech perception to morphology: affix-ordering revisited. Language 78: 527–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, and Baayen, Harald. 2002. Parsing and productivity. Yearbook of Morphology 2001, 203–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, and Bresnan, Joan. 2006. Spoken syntax: the phonetics of giving a hand in New Zealand English. The Linguistic Review 23(3): 321–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, Bernd. 1993. Auxiliaries: cognitive forces and grammaticalization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heine, Bernd, and Kuteva, Tania. 2002. World lexicon of grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, Bernd, and Kuteva, Tania. 2007. The genesis of grammar: a reconstruction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heine, Bernd, and Reh, Mechthild. 1984. Grammaticalization and reanalysis in African languages. Hamburg: H Buske.Google Scholar
Heine, Bernd, Claudi, Ulrike and Hünnemeyer, Friederike. 1991. Grammaticalization: a conceptual framework. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, Sebastian. 2005. Grammaticalization and English complex prepositions: a corpus-based study. London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hook, Peter Edwin. 1991. The emergence of perfective aspect in Indo-Aryan languages. In E. Closs T. and B. Heine (eds.) Approaches to grammaticalization, Vol. II, 5989. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Hooper, Joan B. 1976. Word frequency in lexical diffusion and the source of morphophonological change. In Christie, W. (ed.), Current progress in historical linguistics, 96–105. Amsterdam: North Holland. Reprinted in Bybee 2007: 23–34.Google Scholar
Hooper, Joan B. 1979. Child morphology and morphophonemic change. Linguistics17: 21–50. Reprinted in J. Fisiak (ed.) 1980. Historical morphology, 157–87. The Hague: Mouton.
Hooper, Joan B. and Thompson, Sandra A.. 1973. On the applicability of root transformations. Linguistic Inquiry 4: 465–97.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. 1987. Emergent grammar. Berkeley Linguistic Society 13: 139–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. 1991. On some principles of grammaticization. In Traugott, E. C. and Heine, B. (eds.), Approaches to grammaticalization, Vol.I, 17–35. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. 1994. Phonogenesis. In Pagliuca, W. (ed.), Perspectives on grammaticalization, 29–45. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. 2008. Emergent serialization in English: pragmatics and typology. In Good, J. (ed.) Language universals and language change, 253–84. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J., and Thompson, Sandra A.. 1980. Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language 56: 251–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J., and Thompson, Sandra A.. 1984. The discourse basis for lexical categories in universal grammar. Language 60(4): 703–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J., and Traugott, Elizabeth. 2003. Grammaticalization (2nd edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddleston, Rodney D., and Pullum, Geoffrey K.. 2002. The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Israel, Michael. 1996. The way constructions grow. In Goldberg, A. E. (ed.), Conceptual structure, discourse, and language, 217–30. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 1975. Morphological and semantic regularities in the lexicon. Language 51: 639–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1957 [1971]. Shifters, verbal categories and the Russian verb. Reprinted in Roman Jakobson, Selected Writings II, 130–47. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1966 [1971]. Quest for the essence of language. Diogenes, 51. Reprinted in Roman Jakobson, Selected Writings II, 345–59. The Hague: Mouton.
Jakobson, Roman. 1990. Some questions of meaning. In Waugh, L. R. (ed.), On language: Roman Jakobson, 315–23. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
James, William. 1950 [1890]. Principles of psychology. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Janda, Richard D. 2001. Beyond ‘pathways’ and ‘unidirectionality’: on the discontinuity of language transmission and the counterability of grammaticalization. In Campbell, L. (ed.), Grammaticalization: a critical assessment. Special issue of Language Sciences, 23(2–3): 265–340.
Jespersen, Otto. 1942. A modern English grammar on historical principles, Part VI: Morphology. London: George Allen & Unwin; Copenhagen: Ejnar Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Johnson, Keith. 1997. Speech perception without speaker normalization. In Johnson, K. and Mullennix, J. W. (eds.), Talker variability in speech processing, 145–65. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P., and Stevenson, R.. 1970. Memory for syntax. Nature 227: 412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P., Robins, C. and Velicogna, L.. 1974. Memory for words. Nature 251: 704–705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurafsky, Daniel 1996. A probabilistic model of lexical and syntactic access and disambiguation. Cognitive Science 20: 137–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel, Bell, Alan and Girand, Cynthia. 2002. The role of the lemma in form variation. In Gussenhoven, C. and Warner, N. (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VII, 1–34. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel, Bell, Alan, Gregory, Michelle and Raymond, William. 2001. Probabilistic relations between words: evidence from reduction in lexical production. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 229–54. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kay, Paul. 1977. Language evolution and speech style. In Blount, B. and Sanchez, M. (eds.), Sociocultural dimensions of language change, 21–33. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, Edward L. 1975. Variation in universal grammar. In Keenan, E., Fasold, R. and Shuy, R. (eds.), Analyzing variation in language, 136–48. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Keenan, Edward L., and Comrie, Bernard. 1977. Noun phrase accessibility and Universal Grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 8(1): 63–99.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 1968. Linguistic universals and linguistic change. In Bach, E. and Harms, R.T. (eds.), Universals in linguistic theory, 171–204. New York: Holt Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 1985. Some consequences of lexical phonology. Phonology yearbook 2: 85–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 1995. The phonological basis of sound change. In Goldsmith, J. (ed.), The handbook of phonological theory, 640–70. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kirby, Simon. 2000. Syntax without natural selection: how compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In Knight, C., Hurford, J. & Kennedy, M. Studdert- (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form, 303–23. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirby, Simon. 2001. Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure: an iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Journal of Evolutionary Computation 5(2):102–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, Simon, and Christiansen, Morten. 2003. From language learning to language evolution. In Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language evolution, 279–94. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Klein, Wolfgang, and Perdue, Clive. 1997. The basic variety (or: couldn't natural languages be much simpler?). Second Language Research 13(4): 301–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, Klaus-Michael. 1988. Schemas in German plural formation. Lingua 74: 303–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotovsky, Laura, and Gentner, Dedre. 1996. Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development 67: 2797–822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroch, Anthony. 1989a. Function and grammar in the history of English: periphrastic do. In Fasold, R. W. and Schiffren, D. (eds.), Language change and variation, 134–69. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kroch, Anthony. 1989b. Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change. Language Variation and Change 1: 199–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroch, Anthony, Myhill, John and Pintzuk, Susan. 1982. Understanding do. CLS18: 282–94. Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.
Krott, Andrea, Baayen, Harald and Schreuder, R.. 2001. Analogy in morphology: modeling the choice of linking morphemes in Dutch. Linguistics 39(1): 51–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krug, Manfred. 1998. String frequency: a cognitive motivating factor in coalescence, language processing and linguistic change. Journal of English Linguistics 26: 286–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. 1966. The social stratification of English in New York City. Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1982. Building on empirical foundations. In Lehmann, Winfred P. and Malkiel, Yakov (eds.), Perspectives on historical linguistics, 17–92. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1994. Principles of linguistic change: internal factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancelot, C. and Arnauld, A.. 1660. Grammaire générale et raisonnée. Paris: Pierre le Petit.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 1978. The form and meaning of the English auxiliary. Language 54: 853–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: theoretical prerequisites, Vol.I. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 2000. A dynamic usage-based model. In Barlow, M. and Kemmer, S. (eds.), Usage-based models of language, 1–63. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, Diane. 1997. Chaos/complexity science and second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics 18: 141–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, Christian. 1982. Thoughts on grammaticalization: a programmatic sketch, Vol. I. (Arbeiten des Kölner Universalien-Projekts 48). Köln: Universität zu Köln. Institut für Sprachwissenschaft.Google Scholar
Li, Charles N. 1975. Word order and word order change. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Li, Charles N. 1976. Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Li, Charles N. (ed.) 1977. Mechanisms of syntactic change. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Li, Charles N. 2002. Some issues concerning the origin of language. In Bybee, J. and Noonan, M. (eds.), Complex sentences in grammar and discourse: essays in honor of Sandra A. Thompson, 203–21. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Li, P., Elizabeth Bates and MacWhinney, Brian. 1993. Processing a language without inflections: a reaction time study of sentence interpretation in Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language 32: 169–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Safford Harris, K., Hoffman, H. and Griffith, B. C.. 1957. The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology 54: 358–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenberk, Frantiek. 1991. On the gradualness of grammaticalization. In Traugott, E.C. and Heine, B. (eds.), Approaches to grammaticalization, 37–80. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Lieven, Elena, Pine, Julian M. and Baldwin, Gillian. 1997. Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language 24: 187–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lightfoot, David. 1979. Principles of diachronic syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, David. 1991. How to set parameters: arguments from language change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lin, Zi-Yu. 1991. The development of grammatical markers in Archaic Chinese and Han Chinese. Dissertation. SUNY, Buffalo, NY.Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn. 1990. Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H&H theory. In Hardcastle, W. J. and Marchal, A. (eds.), Speech production and speech modelling, 403–39. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn, MacNeilage, Peter and Studdert-Kennedy, Michael. 1984. Self-organizing processes and the explanation of language universals. In Butterworth, B., Comrie, B. and Dahl, Ö. (eds.), Explanations for language universals, 181–203. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lord, Carol. 1976. Evidence for syntactic reanalysis: from verb to complementizer in Kwa. In Steever, S. B., Walker, C. A. and Mufwene, S. (eds.), Papers from the parasession on diachronic syntax, 179–91. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Los, Bettelou. 2005. The rise of the to-infinitive. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Losiewicz, Beth L. 1992. The effect of frequency on linguistic morphology. Dissertation. University of Texas, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
Luce, Paul, Pisoni, David, and Goldinger, Stephen. 1990. Similarity neighborhoods of spoken words. In Altman, G.n (ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: psycholinguistic and computational perspectives, 122–47. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacFarland, T., and Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1991. On ich-Laut, ach-Laut and structure preservation. Phonology 8: 171–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian. 1978. The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 174(43).Google Scholar
Malt, B. C., and Smith, E. E.. 1984. Correlated properties in natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23: 250–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mańczak, Witold. 1980. Laws of analogy. In Fisiak, J. (ed.), Historical morphology, 28388. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Marchese, Lynell. 1986. Tense/aspect and the development of auxiliaries in Kru languages. Arlington, VA: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Marcos Marín, Francisco 1992. Corpus oral de referencia del español contemporáneo. Textual corpus, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
Marcus, Gary F., Steven Pinker, M. Ullman, M. Hollander, Rosen, T. J. and Xu, F.. 1992. Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the society for research in child development 57(4): 1–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, James L., and Bybee, Joan. 2007. Gradience of gradience: a reply to Jackendoff. The Linguistic Review 24: 437–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McWhorter, John. 2001. Defining ‘creole’ as a synchronic term. In Neumann-Holzschuh, I. and Schneider, E. (eds.), Degrees of restructuring in creole languages, 85–124. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Medin, Douglas. L., and Schaffer, Marguerite M.. 1978. Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review 85: 207–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meillet, Antoine. 1912. L'évolution des formes grammaticales. Scientia (Rivista di scienza) 6(12): 384–400.Google Scholar
Michaelis, Laura A. 2004. Type shifting in Construction Grammar: an integrated approach to aspectual coercion. Cognitive Linguistics 15(1): 1–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michaelis, Laura A. 2006. Tense in English. In Aarts, B. and MacMahon, A. (eds.), The handbook of English linguistics, 220–34. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Miller, George A. 1956. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63: 81–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, Joanne. 1994. On the internal structure of phonetic categories: a progress report. Cognition 50: 271–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moonwomon, Birch. 1992. The mechanism of lexical diffusion. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, January, Philadelphia.
Morton, J. 1967. A singular lack of incidental learning. Nature 215: 203–04.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mossé, Fernand. 1952. A handbook of Middle English. Translated by Walker, James A.. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Mossé, Fernand. 1968. Manual of Middle English. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Mowrey, Richard, and Pagliuca, William. 1995. The reductive character of articulatory evolution. Rivista di Linguistica 7(1): 37–124.Google Scholar
Munson, Benjamin, and Solomon, Nancy P.. 2004. The effect of phonological neighborhood density on vowel articulation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47: 1048–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, G. L., and Shapiro, A. M.. 1994. Forgetting of verbatim information in discourse. Memory and Cognition 22: 85–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nader, K., Schafe, G.E., and Doux, J.E.. 2000. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdale for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature 406: 722–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagle, Stephen J. 1989. Inferential change and syntactic modality in English. Frankfurt am Main: Lang.Google Scholar
Newell, Allen. 1990. Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, Frederick J. 1998. Language form and language function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, Fritz J. 2005. Possible and probable languages: a generative perspective on linguistic typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noonan, Michael. 1998. Non-structuralist syntax. In Darnell, M., Moravcsik, E., Newmeyer, F., Noonan, M. and Wheatley, K. (eds.), Functionalism and formalism in linguistics, Vol. I, 11–31. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Norde, Muriel. 2001. Deflexion as a counterdirectional factor in grammatical change. In Campbell, L. (ed.), Grammaticalization: a critical assessment. Special issue of Language Sciences23(2–3): 231–64.
Nosofsky, Robert M. 1988. Similarity, frequency, and category representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: learning, memory, and cognition 14: 54–65.Google Scholar
Nunberg, Geoffrey, Sag, Ivan A. and Wasow, Thomas. 1994. Idioms. Language 70: 491–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogura, M. 1993. The development of periphrastic do in English: a case of lexical diffusion in syntax. Diachronica 10(1): 51–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, John 1999. Electronic Texts and Concordances of the Madison Corpus of Early Spanish Manuscripts and Printings, CD-ROM. Madison and New York; Hispanic Seminary of Medieval Studies.
Patterson, Janet L. 1992. The development of sociolinguistic phonological variation patterns for (ing) in young children. Dissertation. University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM.Google Scholar
Pawley, Andrew, and Syder, Frances Hodgetts. 1983. Two puzzles for linguistic theory: nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In Richards, J. C. and Schmidt, R. W., Language and communication, 191–226. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Perkins, Revere. 1992. Deixis, grammar, and culture. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, Ann M. 1983. The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1994. Syllable structure and word structure: a study of triconsonantal clusters in English. In Keating, Patricia, ed., Phonological structure and phonetic form: papers in laboratory phonology III, 168–90. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet. 2001. Exemplar dynamics: word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 137–57. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2002. Word-specific phonetics. In Gussenhoven, C. and Warner, N. (eds.), Laboratory phonology 7, 101–39. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2003. Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Language and speech 46(2–3): 115–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pine, Julian M., and Lieven, Elena. 1993. Reanalysing rote-learned phrases: individual differences in the transition to multiword speech. Journal of Child Language 20: 551–71.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1991. Rules of language. Science 253: 530–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, Steven. 1999. Words and rules. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 2003. The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven, and Bloom, Paul. 1990. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, Betty S. 1984. Word frequency and the actuation of sound change. Language 60: 320–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, Betty S. 2001. Lexical diffusion, lexical frequency, and lexical analysis. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 123–36. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Plank, Frans. 1984. The modals story retold. Studies in Language 8(3): 305–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poplack, Shana. To appear. A variationist perspective on grammaticalization. In Heine, B. and Narrog, H. (eds.) Handbook of grammaticalization. Oxford University Press.
Poplack, Shana, and Tagliamonte, Sali. 1996. Nothing in context: variation, grammaticization and past time marking in Nigerian Pidgin English. In Baker, P. and Syea, A. (eds.), Changing meanings, changing functions: papers relating to grammaticalization in contact languages, 71–94. London: University of Westminster.Google Scholar
Pountain, Christopher J. 1984. How ‘become’ became in Castilian. In Essays in honour of Robert Brian Tate from his colleagues and pupils, 101–11. University of Nottingham Monographs in the Humanities.
Prévost, P., and White, L.. 2000. Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second language research 16(2): 103–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quirk, Randolf, Greenbaum, Sydney, Leech, Geoffrey, and Svartvik, Jan. 1985. A comprehensive grammar of the English language. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Reid, Wallis. 1991. Verb and noun number in English: a functional explanation. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Reyna, V. F., and Kiernan, B.. 1994. The development of gist versus verbatim memory in sentence recognition: effects of lexical familiarity, semantic content, encoding instruction, and retention interval. Developmental Psychology 30: 178–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Ian. 1985. Agreement parameters and the development of the English modal auxiliaries. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3: 21–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Ian, and Roussou, Anna. 2003. Syntactic change: a minimalist approach to grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Julie. 1994. Acquisition of variable rules: (-t, d) deletion and (ing) production in preschool children. Dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.
Roberts, Julie. 1997. Acquisition of variable rules: a study of (-t, d) deletion in preschool children. Journal of Child Language 24: 351–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Sarah J., and Bresnan, Joan. 2008. Retained inflectional morphology in pidgins: a typological study. Linguistic Typology 12: 269–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne. 1995. The grammaticalization of irrealis in Tok Pisin. In Bybee, J. and Fleischmann, S. (eds.), Modality in grammar and discourse, 389–427. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor H. 1973. Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 4: 328–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor H. 1975. Cognitive representation of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology 104: 573–605.Google Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor H. 1978. Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E. H. and Lloyd, B. B. (eds.), Cognition and categorization, 27–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, David E., McClelland, James L., and the PDP research group. 1986. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vols. 1–2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline S. 1967. Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse. Perception and Psychophysics 2(9): 437–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian, and Hélène, Blondeau. 2007. Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal French. Language 83: 560–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, Edward. 1921. Language: an introduction to the study of speech. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Savage, Ceri, Lieven, Elena, Theakston, Anna and Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Testing the abstractness of children's linguistic representations: lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science 6(5): 557–567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheibman, Joanne. 2000. I dunno but … a usage-based account of the phonological reduction ofdon't. Journal of Pragmatics 32: 105–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheibman, Joanne 2002. Point of view and grammar: structural patterns of subjectivity in American English conversation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwenter, Scott A. 1994. The grammaticalization of an anterior in progress: evidence from a Peninsular Spanish dialect. Studies in Language 18: 71–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seppänen, Aimo, Bowen, Rhonwen, and Trotta, Joe. 1994. On the so-called complex prepositions. Studia Anglia Posnaniensia 29: 3–29.Google Scholar
Sienicki, Ben. 2008. The dare and need constructions in English: a case of degrammaticization? Unpublished manuscript. University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM.
Siewierska, Anna. 2002. Word order. In Smelser, N. and Baltes, P. (eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (16552–5). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Sinclair, John. 1991. Corpus, concordance, collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Skousen, Royal. 1989. Analogical modeling of language. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1977. Language change in childhood and in history. In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language learning and thought, 185–214. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1985. Cross-linguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition: theoretical perspectives, Vol.II, 1157–256. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1994. Talking perfectly: discourse origins of the Present Perfect. In Pagliuca, W. (ed.), Perspectives on grammaticalization, 119–33. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1996. From “thought” and “language” to “thinking for speaking.” In Gumperz, J.J. and Levinson, S.C. (eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity, 70–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1997a. Mind, code, and text. In Bybee, J., Haiman, J. and Thompson, S. (eds.), Essays on language function and language type, 437–67. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1997b. The origins of grammaticizable notions: beyond the individual mind. In Slobin, D. (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition: expanding the contexts, Vol.V, 1–39. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 2003. Language and thought online: cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in mind: advances in the investigation of language and thought, 157–91. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, Geoff P. 2002. Growing up with Tok Pisin: contact, creolization, and change in Papua New Guinea's national language. London: Battlebridge.Google Scholar
Smith, K. Aaron. 2001. The role of frequency in the specialization of the English anterior. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 361–82. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Smith, Carlota S. 1997. The parameter of aspect. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steele, Susan. 1975. Past and irrealis: just what does it all mean? International Journal of American Linguistics 41: 200–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefanowitsch, Anatol, and Gries, Stefan. 2003. Collostructions: investigating the interaction of words and constructions. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 8(2): 209–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, , Michael, Alvin Liberman, Harris, Katherine and Cooper, Franklin. 1970. Motor theory of speech perception: a reply to Lane's critical review. Psychological Review 77: 234–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taeymans, Martine. 2004. What the Helsinki Corpus tells us about DARE in late Middle English to Early Modern English. Paper presented at 13 ICEHL, University of Vienna, August.
Taeymans, Martine. 2006. An investigation into the emergence and development of the verb need from Old to Present-Day English: a corpus-based approach. Dissertation. University of Antwerp, Belgium.
Talmy, Leonard 1985. Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, Timothy (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, Vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, 57–149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tao, Hongyin. 2003. A usage-based approach to argument structure: ‘remember’ and ‘forget’ in spoken English. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 8(1): 75–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, John. 1995. Linguistic categorization (2nd edn), Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, Sandra A. 1988. A discourse approach to the cross-linguistic category “adjective”. In Hawkins, J. A. (ed.), Explaining language universals, 167–85. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Thompson, Sandra A. 1998. A discourse explanation for the cross-linguistic differences in the grammar of interrogation and negation. In Siewierska, A. and Song, J. J. (eds.), Case, typology and grammar, 309–41. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Thompson, Sandra, and Fox, Barbara. 1990. A discourse explanation of the grammar of relative clauses in English conversation. Language 66(2): 297–316.Google Scholar
Thompson, Sandra A., and Hopper, Paul J.. 2001. Transitivity, clause structure, and argument structure: evidence from conversation. In Bybee, J. and Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 27–60. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Thurston, William R. 1989. How exoteric languages build a lexicon: esoterogeny in West New Britain. In Harlow, R. and Hooper, R. (eds.), VICAL 1: Oceanic languages, Papers from the Fifth International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics, 555–79. Auckland: Linguistic Society of New Zealand.Google Scholar
Tiersma, Peter. 1982. Local and general markedness. Language 58: 832–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 1992. First verbs: a case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, A. Kruger, and Ratner, H.. 1993. Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlin, Russell S. 1986. Basic word order: functional principles. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, Rena. 1999. Variation and grammaticization in progressives: Spanish -ndo constructions. Studies in Language 23(1): 25–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, Rena. 2000. Grammaticization, synchronic variation, and language contact: a study of Spanish progressive -ndo constructions. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, Rena. 2001. From lexical to grammatical to social meaning. Language in Society 30: 443–78.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, Rena. 2006. Relative frequency in the grammaticization of collocations: nominal to concessive a pesar de. In Face, T. and Klee, C. (eds.), Selected proceedings of the 8th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, 37–49. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, Rena, and Schwenter, Scott. 2005. Towards an operational notion of subjectification. Berkeley Linguistics Society 31: 347–58.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, Rena and Walker, James A.. 2009. The present of the English future: grammatical variation and collocations in discourse. Language 85(2) 321–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tottie, Gunnel. 1991. Lexical diffusion in syntactic change: frequency as a determinant of linguistic conservatism in the development of negation in English. In Kastovsky, D. (ed.), Historical English syntax, 439–67. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Trask, Robert. L. 2007. Historical linguistics (2nd edn.). Revised by Millar, Robert McColl. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 1972. A history of English syntax. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 1989. On the rise of epistemic meanings in English: an example of subjectification in semantic change. Language 65: 31–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2001. Legitimate counterexamples to unidirectionality. Paper presented at Freiberg University, October.
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2003. Constructions in grammaticalization. In Joseph, B. and Janda, R. (eds.), A handbook of historical linguistics, 624–47. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and Dasher, Richard B.. 2002. Regularity in semantic change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and König, Ekkehard. 1991. The semantics-pragmatics of grammaticalization revisited. In Traugott, E.C. and Heine, B. (eds.), Approaches to grammaticalization, Vol.I, 189–218. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. 2001. Contact and simplification. Linguistic Typology 5: 371–74.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. 1989. Contact and isolation in linguistic change. In Breivik, L. and Jahr, E. (eds.), Language change: contributions to the study of its causes, 227–37. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. 2002. Linguistic and social typology. In Chambers, J., Trudgill, P. and Schilling-Estes, N. (eds.), Handbook of language variation and change, 707–28. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bergem, Dick. 1995. Acoustic and lexical vowel reduction, Studies in language and language use, 16. Amsterdam: IFOTT.Google Scholar
Gelderen, Elly. 2004. Grammaticalization as economy. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Zeno. 1967. Verbs and times. In Vendler, Z. (ed.), Linguistics in philosophy, 97–121. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Verhagen, Arie. 2002. From parts to wholes and back again. Cognitive Linguistics 13: 403–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhagen, Arie. 2006. English constructions from a Dutch perspective: where are the differences? In Hannay, M. and Steen, G. J. (eds.), Structural-functional studies in English grammar, 257–74. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Vihman, Marilyn. 1980. Sound change and child language. In Papers from the 4th International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, Michael S., Luce, Paul A., Charles-Luce, Jan and Kemmerer, David. 1997. Phonotactics and syllable stress: implications for the processing of spoken nonsense words. Language and Speech, 40: 47–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warner, Anthony. 1983. Review article of Lightfoot 1979 (Principles of diachronic syntax). Journal of Linguistics 19: 187–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, Anthony. 2004. What drove ‘do’? In C. Kay, S. Horobin, and J. J. Smith, (eds.), New perspectives on English historical linguistics: syntax and morphology, Vol.I, 229–42. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Watkins, Calvin. 1962. Indo-European origins of the Celtic verb I: the sigmatic aorist. Dublin: Dublin Institute for Advanced Studies.Google Scholar
Waugh, Linda. 1975. A semantic analysis of the French tense system. Orbis 24: 436–85.Google Scholar
Wedel, Andrew B. 2006. Exemplar models, evolution and language change. The Linguistic Review 23(3): 247–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wedel, Andrew B. 2007. Feedback and regularity in the lexicon. Phonology 24: 147–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whorf, Benjamin Lee. 1941 [1956]. Language, mind, and reality. In Carroll, J. B. (ed.), Language, thought, and reality: selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf, 134–59. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, Damián Vergara. 2009. From ‘remaining’ to ‘becoming’ in Spanish: the role of prefabs in the development of the construction quedar(se) + ADJECTIVE. In Corrigan, R., Moravcsik, E., Ouali, H. and Wheatley, K. (eds.), Formulaic language, Vol.I, Typological studies in language, 273–96. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Wittgenstein, Ludwig 1953. Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wray, Alison 2000. Holistic utterances in protolanguage: the link from primates to humans. In Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J., The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: social function and the origins of linguistic form, 285–302. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wray, Alison 2002. Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, Allison, and Grace, George W.. 2007. The consequences of talking to strangers: evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua 117: 543–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegeler, Debra. 2004. Grammaticalisation through constructions: the story of causative have in English. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 2: 159–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwicky, Arnold, and Pullum, Geoffrey. 1983. Cliticization vs. inflection: Englishn't. Language 59: 502–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Joan Bybee, University of New Mexico
  • Book: Language, Usage and Cognition
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511750526.013
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Joan Bybee, University of New Mexico
  • Book: Language, Usage and Cognition
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511750526.013
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Joan Bybee, University of New Mexico
  • Book: Language, Usage and Cognition
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511750526.013
Available formats
×