We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Edited by
David Mabey, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine,Martin W. Weber, World Health Organization,Moffat Nyirenda, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine,Dorothy Yeboah-Manu, Noguchi Memorial Institute for Medical Research, University of Ghana,Jackson Orem, Uganda Cancer Institute, Kampala,Laura Benjamin, University College London,Michael Marks, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine,Nicholas A. Feasey, Liverpool School of Tropical Medicine
There are approximately 25 major helminth (worm) infections of humans that all, to some extent, have public health significance, but amongst the most common of all human infections are the intestinal nematodes (also called soil-transmitted helminths; STH): the roundworm, Ascaris lumbricoides; the hookworms, Necator americanus and Ancylostoma duodenale; and the whipworm, Trichuris trichiura. The burden of other intestinal nematodes, including Strongyloides stercoralis and Enterobius vermicularis, are ill-defined due to practical difficulties in diagnosing sub-clinical infections. Cestodes are also of major public health and economic importance.
Patients with hematological malignancies are at high risk of infections due to both the disease and the associated treatments. The use of immunoglobulin (Ig) to prevent infections is increasing in this population, but its cost effectiveness is unknown. This trial-based economic evaluation aimed to compare the cost effectiveness of prophylactic Ig with prophylactic antibiotics in patients with hematological malignancies.
Methods
The economic evaluation used individual patient data from the RATIONAL feasibility trial, which randomly assigned 63 adults with chronic lymphocytic leukemia, multiple myeloma, or lymphoma to prophylactic Ig or prophylactic antibiotics. The following two analyses were conducted to estimate the cost effectiveness of the two treatments over the 12-month trial period from the perspective of the Australian health system:
(i) a cost-utility analysis (CUA) to assess the incremental cost per quality-adjusted life-year (QALY) gained using data collected with the EuroQol 5D-5L questionnaire; and
(ii) a cost-effectiveness analysis (CEA) to assess the incremental cost per serious infection prevented (grade ≥3) and per infection prevented (any grade).
Results
The total cost per patient was significantly higher in the Ig arm than in the antibiotic arm (difference AUD29,140 [USD19,000]). There were non-significant differences in health outcomes between the treatment arms: patients treated with Ig had fewer QALYs (difference −0.072) and serious infections (difference −0.26) than those given antibiotics, but more overall infections (difference 0.76). The incremental cost-effectiveness from the CUA indicated that Ig was more costly than antibiotics and associated with fewer QALYs. In the CEA, Ig costed an additional AUD111,262 (USD73,000) per serious infection prevented, but it was more costly than antibiotics and associated with more infections when all infections were included.
Conclusions
These results indicate that, on average, Ig prophylactic treatment may not be cost effective compared with prophylactic antibiotics for the group of patients with hematological malignancies recruited to the RATIONAL feasibility trial. Further research is needed to confirm these findings in a larger population and over the longer term.
Product architecture decisions are made early in the product development process and have far-reaching effects. Unless anticipated through experience or intuition, many of these effects may not be apparent until much later in the development process, making changes to the architecture costly in time, effort and resources. Many researchers through the years have studied various elements of product architecture and their effects. By using a repeatable process for aggregating statements on the effects of architecture strategies from a selection of the literature on the topic and storing them in a systematic database, this information can then be recalled and presented in the form of a Product Architecture Strategy and Effect (PASE) matrix. PASE matrices allow for the identification, comparison, evaluation, and then selection of the most desirable product architecture strategies before expending resources along a specific development path. This paper introduces the PASE Database and matrix and describes their construction and use in guiding design decisions. This paper also provides metrics for understanding the robustness of this database.
Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) type 1 (HIV-1), cardiovascular disease, and HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) disproportionately affect Black/African American individuals compared to other racial and ethnic groups. Understanding the mechanisms of cognitive health disparities is essential for developing policy and health interventions to combat such disparities. Cardiovascular risk factors/diseases are common comorbidities that likely contribute to cognitive health disparities among Black/African American people living with HIV (PWH), but their impacts on cognition longitudinally in this population are unclear. The current study examines the relationship between cardiovascular risk and cognitive functioning over time in Black/African American adults living with HIV.
Participants and Methods:
A sample of 122 Black/African American adults with HIV (ages 25-68, M=51.8, SD=7.7; 98% on antiretroviral therapy; 91% with undetectable viral load) were selected from the Drexel/Temple Comprehensive NeuroHIV Center, Clinical and Translational Research Support Core (CTRSC; based at Drexel University College of Medicine) Cohort. They completed longitudinal visits (300 total visits, average follow-up time=4.9 years) that included clinical interviews, medical record review, biometric measurements, and comprehensive neuropsychological assessments. Cardiovascular risk factors of interest were body mass index (BMI), waist-to-height ratio (WHtR), and a total vascular risk burden score (VBS) representing five risk factors: obesity, central obesity, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, and hypertension. Based on a prior principal component analysis, three cognitive domains were examined: (1) verbal fluency, (2) visual memory/visuoconstruction, and (3) motor speed/executive functions. Mixed models were used to examine domain-specific cognitive trajectories in relation to baseline cardiovascular risk factors and changes in cardiovascular risk factors.
Results:
Overall, cognitive test performance improved over time (p<.003). Baseline VBS was marginally associated with longitudinal change in verbal fluency (p=.06). Participants with low baseline VBS (0-1 risk factors) demonstrated improvement in verbal fluency (p=.002), while those with higher VBS (2-5 risk factors) demonstrated stability in verbal fluency. In contrast, greater increases in BMI and in WHtR predicted more favorable trajectories in motor speed/executive function (both p<.001). Patients with increasing BMI over time improved in this domain (p=.02), while patients with stable or decreasing BMI did not. A similar pattern was observed for WHtR change. No vascular risk factors were associated with trajectories of visual memory/visuoconstruction.
Conclusions:
Higher total vascular risk burden was associated with less favorable verbal fluency trajectories, reflecting the negative cognitive consequences of disorders such as diabetes, hyperlipidemia, and hypertension. Unexpectedly, greater increases in BMI and WHtR were associated with more favorable trajectories in motor speed and executive functioning. In this population, weight gain may be a proxy for other positive health factors, such as immune reconstitution, which will be examined in future analyses. Taken together, cardiovascular risk factors have heterogeneous associations with cognitive trajectories, emphasizing the importance of examining the mechanisms of these varying relationships. Future research will examine how social determinants of health, such as racial/ethnic discrimination, contribute to disparities in cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive outcomes.
This chapter explores the expansion of transitional justice from present violence to past violence. This has been called post-transitional justice. It considers cases where long neglected victims of colonial repression have reentered history as claimants in controversial legal cases about atrocities in Kenya and Indonesia. The UK High Court decision on compensation for Mau Mau victims of British colonial repression (2010) and compensation for widows of the Rawagede village massacre of 400 villagers in 1947 by Dutch colonial troops in East Java (2011), extended legal accountability for empires, and remedies for individuals. While the globalization of human rights discourse has created the transnational legal environment in which these claims were made possible, it has also exposed the tension between the compromises of national self-determination and individuated justice. By unstitching the problematic settlement of collective national independence, and revealing asymmetries of suffering, it reveals the difficulty of treating political transition as caesura, and the ongoing legacies of trauma which persist well after the departure of the imperial power.
Thinking About Human Memory provides a novel analytical approach to understanding memory that considers the goals of the memory task, the cues and information available, the opportunity to learn, and interference from irrelevant information (noise). Each of the five chapters describing this approach introduces historical ideas and demonstrates how current thinking both differs from and is derived from them. These chapters also contain analyses of current problems designed to demonstrate the power of the approach. In a subsequent chapter, the authors discuss how memory is controlled by the environment, by others, and by ourselves, and then apply their insights to the problem solving of children, our hominin ancestors, and scrub jays. Finally, the questions of how to define episodic memory and how to investigate phylogenetic and developmental changes in memory are addressed. This book will appeal to memory researchers, including applied researchers, and advanced students.