Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T11:56:14.994Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part II - Aptitude Testing of Diverse Groups

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 May 2023

Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Peter Skehan
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, University of London
Richard L. Sparks
Affiliation:
Mount St Joseph University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bokander, L., & Bylund, E. (2020). Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning, 70(1), 1147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1965). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In Glaser, R. (ed.), Training, Research and Education. New York: Wiley, pp. 87136.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1966). A Parametric Study of Language Training in the Peace Corps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 83118.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. M. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test Manual. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale NJ: Erbaum.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2019). The future of language aptitude research. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. London: Routledge, pp. 317329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., & Skehan, P. (2003). Individual differences in second language learning. In Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (eds.), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 589630.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. J. (2019). Cognitive language aptitude. Language Learning, 69(Suppl S1), 101126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrman, M., & Oxford, R. (1995). Cognition plus: Correlates of language learning success. Modern Language Journal, 79(1), 6789.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2013). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA Language Aptitude Test. In Granena, G. & Long, M. H. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 105129.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2016). Cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning and information-processing styles: An individual differences study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(3), 577600.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency: Link between LLAMA and Hi-LAB. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(2), 313336.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Sternberg, R. J., & Ehrman, M. E. (2000). A theory-based approach to the measurement of foreign language learning ability: The Canal-F theory and test. The Modern Language Journal, 84(3), 390405.Google Scholar
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M.. (1992). L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14(1), 2538.Google Scholar
Hatcher, L. (1994). Step-by-Step Approach to Using the SAS System for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.Google Scholar
Hummel, K. M. (2009). Aptitude, phonological memory, and second language proficiency in non-novice adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(2), 225249.Google Scholar
Li, L. R. (2014). Developing an instrument to measure foreign language aptitude of Chinese learners of foreign languages (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China.Google Scholar
Li, L. R., & Luo, S. Q. (2019). Development and preliminary validation of a foreign language aptitude test for Chinese learners of foreign languages. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (eds.). Language aptitude: Advancing theory, testing, research and practice (pp. 59–90). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Li, S. F. (2016). The construct validity of language aptitude: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(4), 801842.Google Scholar
Li, S. F. (2019). Six decades of language aptitude research: A comprehensive and critical review. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York: Routledge, pp. 7896.Google Scholar
Li, S. F., & Qian, J. C. (2021). Exploring syntactic priming as a measure of implicit language aptitude. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263120000698CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., & Campbell, S. G. et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63(3), 530566.Google Scholar
Liu, J., & Jiang, N. (2006) A report on English language aptitude test among Chinese learners. Foreign Languages in China, 3(4), 6368.Google Scholar
Liu, J. D, & Wu, S. (2019). The Research on China’s Standards of English Language Ability. Beijing: Higher Education Press.Google Scholar
Liu, T., Liu, L., & Deng, J. (2005). A study on correlation coefficients of grammar sensitivity. Journal of Southwest Minzu University (Humanities and Social Science), 26(10), 292295.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1995). Aptitude from an information processing perspective. Language Testing, 12(3), 370387.Google Scholar
Meara, P. M. (2005). Llama Language Aptitude Tests. Swansea: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Freidman, N. P. (1998). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In Healy, A. & Bourne, L. (eds.), Foreign Language Learning: Psycholinguistic Studies on Training and Retention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erbaum, pp. 339364.Google Scholar
Petersen, C. R., & Al-Haik, A. R. (1976). The development of the defense language aptitude battery (DLAB). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 36(2), 369380.Google Scholar
Pimsleur, P. (1966). Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength, and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and incidental L2 learning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfield, and Hernstatd (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 235268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitudes, abilities, contexts, and practice. In DeKeyser, R. M. (ed.), Practice in a Second Language: Perspectives from Applied Linguistics and Cognitive Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 256286.Google Scholar
Rogers, V., Meara, P., Barnett-Legh, T., Curry, C., & Davie, E. (2017). Examining the LLAMA aptitude tests. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 1(1), 4960.Google Scholar
Safar, A., & Kormos, J. (2008). Revisiting problems with foreign language aptitude. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 46(2), 113136.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N. (2007). Working memory and L2 processing of redundant grammatical forms. In Han, Z. & Park, E. S. (eds.), Understanding Second Language. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 133147.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J. (2010). The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 120(8), 20022039.Google Scholar
Sawyer, M., & Ranta, L. (2001). Aptitude, individual differences, and instructional design. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 319353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2002). Theorizing and updating aptitude. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 6994.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2012). Language aptitude. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (eds.), Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. New York, NY: Routledge, pp. 381395.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2015). Foreign language aptitude and its relationship with grammar: A critical overview. Applied Linguistics, 36(3), 367384.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., & Patton, J. (1995). Prediction of performance in first-year foreign language courses: Connections between native and foreign language learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(4), 638655.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2011). Subcomponents of second language aptitude and second language proficiency. The Modern Language Journal, 95(2), 253273.Google Scholar
Suzuki, Y. (2021). Probing the construct validity of LLAMA_D as a measure of implicit learning aptitude: Incidental instructions, confidence ratings, and reaction time. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263120000704Google Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2017). The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language: Insights from individual differences in cognitive aptitudes. Language Learning, 67(4), 747790.Google Scholar
Walter, C. (2004). Transfer of reading comprehension skills to L2 is linked to mental representation of text and to L2 working memory. Applied Linguistics, 25(3), 315339.Google Scholar
Wen, Z. S. (2019). Working memory as language aptitude: The Phonological/Executive model. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R., Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. London: Routledge, pp.187214.Google Scholar
Wen, Z. S., Biedroń, A., & Skehan, P. (2017). Foreign language aptitude theory: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Language Teaching, 50(1), 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wen, Z. S., & Skehan, P. (2011). A new perspective on foreign language aptitude research: Building and supporting a case for “working memory as language aptitude.” Ilha do Desterro, 60, 1543.Google Scholar
Weir, C. J. (2005). Language Testing and Validation: An Evidence-Based Approach. New York: Palgrave McMillan.Google Scholar
Winke, P. (2013). An investigation into second language aptitude for advanced Chinese language learning. The Modern Language Journal, 97(1), 109130.Google Scholar
Wu, Y. A., Liu, R, Q., & Jeffrey, P. (1993). Learner factors and language learning achievement: A survey. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 93(1), 3646.Google Scholar
Xia, H. Y. (2011). A language aptitude test for native Chinese: Development and validation. Shandong Foreign Languages Teaching Journal, 32(5), 5255.Google Scholar
Yalçın, S., Cecen, S., & Erçetin, G. (2016). The relationship between aptitude and working memory: An instructed SLA context. Language Awareness, 25(1–2), 144158.Google Scholar
Yalçın, S., & Erçetin, G. (2019). Relationships among working memory, language aptitude, and listening comprehension in an instructed foreign language setting. Zhuhai International Roundtable on Language Aptitude. November 2019.Google Scholar
Yoshimura, Y. (2001). The role of working memory in language aptitude. In Bonch-Bruevich, X., Crawford, W. J., Hellermann, J., Higgins, C., & Nguyen, H. (eds.), The Past, Present, and Future of Second Language Research. Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 144163.Google Scholar

References

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2008). The robustness of aptitude effects in near-native second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(4), 481509.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59(2), 249306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Artieda, G., & Muñoz, C. (2016). The LLAMA tests and the underlying structure of language aptitude at two levels of foreign language proficiency. Learning and Individual Differences, 50, 4248.Google Scholar
Asher, J. J. (1977). Defense language aptitude battery. JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.2307/325703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999-0Google Scholar
Benson, S., & DeKeyser, R. (2019). Effects of written corrective feedback and language aptitude on verb tense accuracy. Language Teaching Research, 23(6), 702726.Google Scholar
Bigelow, M., & Tarone, E. (2004). The role of literacy level in second language acquisition: Doesn’t who we study determine what we know? TESOL Quarterly, 38(4), 689700.Google Scholar
Bokander, L., & Bylund, E. (2020). Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning, 70(1), 1147.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1964). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED038051Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1973). Implications of aptitude test research and psycholinguistic theory for foreign-language teaching. Linguistics, 11(112), 514.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 83118.Google Scholar
Chaffee, K. E., Lou, N. M., & Noels, K. A. (2020). Does stereotype threat affect men in language domains? Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1302. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaffee, K. E., Lou, N. M., Noels, K. A., & Katz, J. W. (2020). Why don’t “real men” learn languages? Masculinity threat and gender ideology suppress men’s language learning motivation. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 23(2), 301318.Google Scholar
Christiner, M., Rüdegger, S., & Reiterer, S. M. (2018). Sing Chinese and tap Tagalog? Predicting individual differences in musical and phonetic aptitude using language families differing by sound-typology. International Journal of Multilingualism, 15(4), 455471.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G., Lynch, J. M., Mendes, N., & Zhai, C. (2019). On bilingual aptitude for learning new languages: The roles of linguistic and nonlinguistic individual differences. Language Learning, 69(2), 478514.Google Scholar
Curcic, M., Andringa, S., & Kuiken, F. (2019). The role of awareness and cognitive aptitudes in L2 predictive language processing. Language Learning, 69, 4271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499533. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100004022Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2012). Age effects in second language learning. In Gass, S. M. & Mackey, A. (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Routledge, pp. 442460.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2019). The future of language aptitude research. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. L. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. Routledge, pp. 317329.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R., Alfi-Shabtay, I., & Ravid, D. (2010). Cross-linguistic evidence for the nature of age effects in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(3), 413438. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000056Google Scholar
del Mar Suárez, M., & Gesa, F. (2019). Learning vocabulary with the support of sustained exposure to captioned video: Do proficiency and aptitude make a difference? The Language Learning Journal, 47(4), 497517. https://doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2019.1617768Google Scholar
Delage, H., & Franck, J. (2021). Diagnostic de troubles du langage chez l’enfant réfugié: Challenges actuels. Babylonia Journal of Language Education, 2, 4451.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., & Skehan, P. (2003). Individual differences in second language learning. In Doughty, C. J. and Long, M. H. (Eds.), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Blackwell, pp. 589630.Google Scholar
Drozdova, P., Van Hout, R., & Scharenborg, O. (2017). L2 voice recognition: The role of speaker-, listener-, and stimulus-related factors. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(5), 30583068.Google Scholar
Escobar, G. P., Kalashnikova, M., & Escudero, P. (2018). Vocabulary matters! The relationship between verbal fluency and measures of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 170, 177189.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2012). Age differences and cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning in ultimate second language attainment. PhD thesis, University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2014). Language aptitude and long-term achievement in early childhood L2 learners. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 483503. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu013Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2016). Cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning and information-processing styles: An individual differences study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(3), 577600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency: Links between LLAMA and Hi-LAB. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(2), 313336.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. H. (2013). Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29(3), 311343.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2018). Aptitude-treatment interaction in L2 learning: A research synthesis. Studies in English Education, 4, 803830.Google Scholar
Hamrick, P. (2015). Declarative and procedural memory abilities as individual differences in incidental language learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 44, 915.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010a). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 2929.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010b). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.Google Scholar
Huang, T., Loerts, H., & Steinkrauss, R. (2020). The impact of second- and third-language learning on language aptitude and working memory. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(2), 117.Google Scholar
Ishikawa, M. (2019). Written languaging, learners’ aptitude and second language learning. PhD thesis, University College London.Google Scholar
Jackson, D. O. (2014). Learner differences in metalinguistic awareness: Exploring the influence of cognitive abilities and language experience. In Miller, R. T., Martin, K. I., Eddington, C. M. et al. (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2012 Second Language Research Forum: Building Bridges between Disciplines, October 18–21, 2012, University of Pittsburgh and Carnegie Mellon University, pp. 211–226. www.lingref.com/cpp/slrf/2012/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Kachinske, I. (2016). The role of rules, examples and individual differences in the acquisition of declarative and procedural second language knowledge. PhD thesis, University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Kourtali, N.-E. (2018). The effects of task complexity, mode of interaction and L2 aptitude on the development of the present third person singular through recasts. PhD thesis, University College London.Google Scholar
Kourtali, N.-E., & Révész, A. (2020). The roles of recasts, task complexity, and aptitude in child second language development. Language Learning, 70(1), 179218.Google Scholar
Lambelet, A. (2021). Lexical diversity development in newly arrived parent–child immigrant pairs: Aptitude, age, exposure, and anxiety. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190521000039Google Scholar
Lambelet, A., & Berthele, R. (2019). Difficulty and ease in foreign language learning at the primary school level: General learning ability, language aptitude or working memory. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. L. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. Routledge, pp. 99122.Google Scholar
Lambelet, A., Berthele, R., & Udry, I. (2019). Les aptitudes langagières: Construit et opérationnalisation: Une revue de la littérature. Institut de plurilinguisme.Google Scholar
Larson-Hall, J., & Dewey, D. (2012). An examination of the effects of input, aptitude, and motivation on the language proficiency of missionaries learning Japanese as a second language. Second Language Acquisition Abroad: The LDS Missionary Experience, 45, 5188.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63(3), 530566.Google Scholar
Ma, D., Yao, T., & Zhang, H. (2018). The effect of third language learning on language aptitude among English-major students in China. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 39(7), 590601.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests: The Manual. University of Wales, Swansea. www.lognostics.co.uk/tools/llama/llama_manual.pdfGoogle Scholar
Medin, D., Ojalehto, B., Marin, A., Bang, M. (2017) Systems of (non)diversity. Nature Human Behaviour 1, Article 0088. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0088CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Métraux, J.-C. (2017). L’impact du vécu des réfugiés sur leur apprentissage de la langue d’accueil. Babylonia, 1, 1922.Google Scholar
Michaud, G. (2020). À quel moment enseigner la forme dans le cadre d’un enseignement basé sur la tâche? PhD thesis, Université de Montréal.Google Scholar
Moon, J. (2012). Maturational and non-maturational factors in heritage language acquisition. PhD thesis, University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Moorman, C. M. (2017). Individual differences and linguistic factors in the development of mid vowels in L2 Spanish learners: A longitudinal study. PhD thesis, Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Okumura, Y., & Okumura, M. Y. (2012). Package ‘rpsychi.’ R package. Published online: http://www2.uaem.mx/r-mirror/web/packages/rpsychi/rpsychi.pdfGoogle Scholar
Parry, T. S., & Child, J. R. (1990). Preliminary investigation of the relationship between VORD, MLAT and language proficiency. Spons Agency, 30–66.Google Scholar
Petersen, C. R., & Al-Haik, A. R. (1976). The development of the defense language aptitude battery (DLAB). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 36(2), 369380.Google Scholar
Portocarrero, J. S., Burright, R. G., & Donovick, P. J. (2007). Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(3), 415422.Google Scholar
Rodríguez Silva, L. H. (2017). The role of cognitive individual differences and learning difficulty in instructed adults’ explicit and implicit knowledge of selected L2 grammar points: A study with Mexican learners of English. PhD thesis, University of Essex.Google Scholar
Rogers, V., Meara, P., Barnett-Legh, T., Curry, C., & Davie, E. (2017). Examining the LLAMA aptitude tests. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 1(1), 4960. www.euroslajournal.org/articles/10.22599/jesla.24/Google Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Salvatierra, J., et al. (2002). A cross-linguistic comparison of verbal fluency tests. International Journal of Neuroscience, 112(6), 759776.Google Scholar
Rüdegger, S. (2017). Language aptitude in primary school children and its effects on other cognitive domains. Master’s thesis, University of Vienna.Google Scholar
Saito, K. (2017). Effects of sound, vocabulary, and grammar learning aptitude on adult second language speech attainment in foreign language classrooms. Language Learning, 67(3), 665693.Google Scholar
Saito, K. (2019). The role of aptitude in second language segmental learning: The case of Japanese learners’ English/ɹ/pronunciation attainment in classroom settings. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(1), 183204.Google Scholar
Saito, K., Sun, H., & Tierney, A. (2018). Explicit and implicit aptitude effects on second language speech learning: Scrutinizing segmental and suprasegmental sensitivity and performance via behavioural and neurophysiological measures. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 118.Google Scholar
Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, M. J. (1966). The Influence of Culture on Visual Perception. Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. IRAL-International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 10(1–4), 209232.Google Scholar
Søndergaard, H.-P. (2017). Language learning in refugees, an account of a Swedish study. Babylonia, 1, 1518.Google Scholar
Tran, M. N. (2019). Scrutinizing the effects of the 4/3/2 activity: Repetition, increasing time pressure, accuracy enhancement and cognitive individual differences. PhD thesis, Birkbeck, University of London.Google Scholar
Turker, S., Seither-Preisler, A., Reiterer, S. M., & Schneider, P. (2019). Cognitive and behavioural weaknesses in children with reading disorder and AD (H) D. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 111.Google Scholar
UN DESA. (2019, September 17). The number of international migrants reaches 272 million, continuing an upward trend in all world regions, says UN. www.un.org/development/desa/en/news/population/international-migrant-stock-2019.htmlGoogle Scholar
Yalçın, Ş., Çeçen, S., & Erçetin, G. (2016). The relationship between aptitude and working memory: An instructed SLA context. Language Awareness, 25(1–2), 144158. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658416.2015.1122026Google Scholar
Yalçın, Ş., & Spada, N. (2016). Language aptitude and grammatical difficulty: An EFL classroom-based study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 239263.Google Scholar
Yilmaz, Y. (2013). Relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback: The role of working memory capacity and language analytic ability. Applied Linguistics, 34(3), 344368.Google Scholar
Yilmaz, Y., & Granena, G. (2016). The role of cognitive aptitudes for explicit language learning in the relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback. Bilingualism, 19(1), 147.Google Scholar

References

Abrahamsson, N. (2012). Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(2), 187214.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. (2008). The robustness of aptitude effects in near-native second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(4), 481509.Google Scholar
Abrahamsson, N. & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59(2), 249306.Google Scholar
Amedi, A., Floel, A., Knecht, S., Zohary, E., & Cohen, L. G. (2004). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the occipital pole interferes with verbal processing in blind subjects. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 12661270.Google Scholar
Amedi, A., Raz, N., Pianka, P., Malach, R., & Zohary, E. (2003). Early “visual” cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 758766.Google Scholar
Ameringer, V., Green, L., Leisser, D., & Turker, S. (2018). Introduction: Towards an interdisciplinary understanding of language aptitude. In Reiterer, S. M. (ed.), Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience. Springer, pp. 115.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: Where and how? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(6), 443452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedny, M. (2017). Evidence from blindness for a cognitively pluripotent cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 637648.Google Scholar
Bedny, M., Pascual-Leone, A., Dodell-Feder, D., Fedorenko, E., & Saxe, R. (2011). Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(11), 44294434.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12(1), 311.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2016). The signal and the noise: Finding the pattern in human behavior. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6(5), 517534.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143(3), 233262.Google Scholar
Bokander, L., & Bylund, E. (2020). Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning, 70(1), 1147.Google Scholar
Bylund, E., Hyltenstam, K., & Abrahamsson, N. (2021). Age of acquisition – not bilingualism – is the primary determinant of less than nativelike L2 ultimate attainment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24(1), 1830.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1958). A factor analysis of two foreign language aptitude batteries. Journal of General Psychology, 59(1), 39.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1962). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In Glaser, R. (ed.), Training Research and Education. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 87136.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1973). Implications of aptitude test research and psycholinguistic theory for foreign language teaching. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 2, 514.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 83118.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. M. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Cenoz, J. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on third language acquisition: Focus on multilingualism. Language Teaching, 46(1), 7186.Google Scholar
Chalmers, J. (2017). How stable is second language aptitude? Effects of second language learning and language analysis training on second language aptitude test scores. Doctoral dissertation, Griffith University, USA.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G., Lynch, J. M., Mendes, N., & Zhai, C. (2019). On bilingual aptitude for learning new languages: The roles of linguistic and nonlinguistic individual differences. Language Learning, 69(2), 478514.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499533.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R., & Koeth, J. (2011). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning. In Hinkel, E. (ed.), Handbook of Research in Second Language Teaching and Learning. New York, NY: Routledge, pp. 395406.Google Scholar
Dick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., et al. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the nationally representative ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 692701.Google Scholar
Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., & Ackermann, H. (2013). Ultra-fast speech comprehension in blind subjects engages primary visual cortex, fusiform gyrus, and pulvinar – a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. BMC Neuroscience 14 , 74.Google Scholar
Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J., & Homer, B. D. (2019). Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 11221147.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. (2019). Cognitive language aptitude. Language Learning, 69(S1), 101126.Google Scholar
Eisenstein, M. (1980). Childhood bilingualism and adult language learning aptitude. International Review of Applied Psychology 29(1–2), 159172.Google Scholar
Forsberg-Lundell, F., & Sandgren, M. (2013). High-level proficiency in late L2 acquisition. Relationships between collocational production, language aptitude and personality. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins, pp. 231258.Google Scholar
Frasnelli, J., Collignon, O., Voss, P., & Lepore, F. (2011). Crossmodal plasticity in sensory loss. Progress in Brain Research, 191, 233249.Google Scholar
Ganschow, L., & Sparks, R. (1995). Effects of direct instruction in Spanish phonology on the native-language skills and foreign-language aptitude of at-risk foreign-language learners. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(2), 107120.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social Psychology and Second Language Learning: The Role of Attitudes and Motivation. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2013). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA language aptitude test. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 105130.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. (2013). Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29(3), 311343.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Sternberg, R. J., & Ehrman, M. E. (2000). A theory-based approach to the measurement of foreign language learning ability: The Canal-F theory and test. The Modern Language Journal, 84(3), 390405.Google Scholar
Harley, B., & Hart, D. (1997). Language aptitude and second language proficiency in classroom learners of different starting ages. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(3), 379400.Google Scholar
Harley, B., & Hart, D. (2002). Age, aptitude, and second language learning on a bilingual exchange. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 301–30.Google Scholar
Herdina, P., & Jessner, U. (2002). A Dynamic Model of Multilingualism: Perspectives of Change in Psycholinguistics. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T. (2017). Direct and indirect effects of multilingualism on novel language learning: An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 892916.Google Scholar
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), F9F15.Google Scholar
Hugdahl, K., Ek, M., Takio, F., et al. (2004). Blind individuals show enhanced perceptual and attentional sensitivity for identification of speech sounds. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(1), 2832.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K. (2021). Language aptitude and language awareness: Polyglot perspectives. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 41, 5575.Google Scholar
Ioup, G., Boustagui, E., El Tigi, M., & Moselle, M. (1994). Reexamining the critical period hypothesis: A case study in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16(1), 7398.Google Scholar
Jessner, U. (2006). Linguistic Awareness in Multilinguals: English as a Third Language. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317324.Google Scholar
Kormos, J. (2013). New conceptualizations of language aptitude in second language attainment. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 131152.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1977). The monitor model of adult second language performance. In Burt, M., Dulay, H., & Finocchiaro, M. (eds.), Viewpoints on English as a Second Language. New York: Regents, pp. 152161.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1981). Aptitude and attitude in relation to second language acquisition and learning. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 155175.Google Scholar
Lamendella, J. T. (1977). General principles of neurofunctional organization and their manifestation in primary and nonprimary language acquisition. Language Learning, 27(1), 155196.Google Scholar
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., et al. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394425.Google Scholar
Leivada, E., Westergaard, M., Andoni Duñabeitia, J., & Rothman, J. (2021). On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition: (How) should we proceed? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(1), 197210.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2016). The construct validity of language aptitude. A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(4), 801842.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2019). Six decades of language aptitude research. A comprehensive and critical review. In Wen, Z. (E.), Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. L. (eds.), Language Aptitude. Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York: Routledge, pp. 7896.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63(3), 530566.Google Scholar
Loiotile, R., Lane, C., Omaki, A., & Bedny, M. (2020). Enhanced performance on a sentence comprehension task in congenitally blind adults. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(8), 10101023.Google Scholar
Lowe, C. J., Cho, I., Goldsmith, S. F., & Morton, J. B. (2021). The bilingual advantage in children’s executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Science, 32(7), 11151146. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797621993108Google Scholar
Ma, D., Yao, T., & Zhang, H. (2018). The effect of third language learning on language aptitude among English-major students in China. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 39(7), 590601.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005a). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005b). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests: The Manual. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Meara, P., Milton, J., & Lorenzo-Dus, N. (2003). Swansea Language Aptitude Tests (LAT) v2.0. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Miller, L. (1992). Diderot reconsidered: Visual impairment and auditory compensation. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 86(5), 206210.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. (1998). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In Healy, A. & Bourne, L. (eds.), Foreign Language Learning: Psycholinguistic Studies on Training and Retention. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 339364.Google Scholar
Novén, M., Olsson, H., Helms, G., et al. (2021). Cortical and white matter correlates of language-learning aptitudes. Human Brain Mapping, 42(15), 50375050 First view: https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25598Google Scholar
Ottó, I. (2002). Magyar Egységes Nyelvérzékmérő-Teszt [Hungarian Language Aptitude Test]. Kaposvár: Mottó-Logic Bt.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R. (2019). The bilingual advantage debate. Quantity and quality of the evidence. In Schwieter, J. W. (ed.), The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism. London: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 701735.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Mason, L., Zimiga, B., Ayala-Silva, Y., & Frost, M. (2020). The alchemy of confirmation bias transmutes expectations into bilingual advantages: A tale of two new meta-analyses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(8), 12901299.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. R., & Al-Haik, A. R. (1976). The development of the Defense Language Aptitude Battery (DLAB). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 36(2), 369380.Google Scholar
Planchon, A., & Ellis, E. (2014). A diplomatic advantage? The effects of bilingualism and formal language training on language aptitude amongst Australian diplomatic officers. Language Awareness, 23(3), 203219.Google Scholar
Ranta, L. (2002). The role of learners’ language analytic ability in the communicative classroom. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 159180.Google Scholar
Raz, N., Amedi, A., & Zohary, E. (2005). V1 activation in congenitally blind humans is associated with episodic retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 15(9), 14591468.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (1997). Individual differences and the fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit adult second language learning. Language Learning, 47(1): 4599.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2002). Effects of individual differences in intelligence, aptitude and working memory on incidental second language learning: A replication and extension of Reber, Walkenfield and Hernstadt (1991). In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 212266.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 4673.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2007). Aptitudes, abilities, contexts, and practice. In DeKeyser, R. (ed.), Practice in a Second Language: Perspectives from Applied Linguistics and Cognitive Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 256286.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2012). Individual differences, aptitude complexes, SLA processes, and aptitude test development. In Pawlak, M. (ed.), New Perspectives on Individual Differences in Language Learning and Teaching. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 5775.Google Scholar
Röder, B., & Neville, H. J. (2003). Developmental functional plasticity. In Grafman, J. & Robertson, I. H. (eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology. Oxford: Elsevier Science, pp. 231270.Google Scholar
Röder, B., Rösler, F., & Neville, H. J. (2000). Event-related potentials during auditory language processing in congenitally blind and sighted people. Neuropsychologia, 38(11), 14821502.Google Scholar
Röder, B., Rösler, F., & Neville, H. J. (2001). Auditory memory in congenitally blind adults: A behavioral-electrophysiological investigation. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(2), 289303.Google Scholar
Roehr-Brackin, K., & Tellier, A. (2019). The role of language-analytic ability in children’s instructed second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(5), 11111131.Google Scholar
Rogers, V., Meara, P., Barnett-Legh, T., Curry, C., & Davie, E. (2017). Examining the LLAMA aptitude tests. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 1, 4960.Google Scholar
Rokem, A., & Ahissar, M. (2009). Interactions of cognitive and auditory abilities in congenitally blind individuals. Neuropsychologia, 47(3), 843848.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. (1983). Patterns of interaction between three blind infants and their mothers. In Mills, A. E. (ed.), Language Acquisition in the Blind Child: Normal and Deficient. San Diego: College Hill Press, pp. 114132.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. (1984). Preverbal communication of blind infants and their mothers. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 78(7), 297302.Google Scholar
Sáfár, A., & Kormos, J. (2008). Revisiting problems with foreign language aptitude. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 46(2), 113136.Google Scholar
Sawyer, M. (1992). Language aptitude and language experience: Are they related? The Language Programs of the International University of Japan Working Papers, 3, 2745.Google Scholar
Sawyer, M., & Ranta, L. (2001). Aptitude, individual differences, and instructional design. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 319353.Google Scholar
Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language teaching, 10(3), 209231.Google Scholar
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 45(1), 2150.Google Scholar
Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(2), 155172.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (2014). Apt to change: The problematic of language awareness and language aptitude in age-related research. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 4(3), 557571.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (2017). Language aptitude: Desirable trait or acquirable attribute? Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 7(1), 89103.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual Differences in Second Language Learning. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2002). Theorising and updating aptitude. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 6995.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2012). Language aptitude. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (eds.), Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. London: Routledge, pp. 381395.Google Scholar
Smeds, H. (2015). Blindness and Second Language Acquisition. Studies of Cognitive Advantages in Blind L1 and L2 Speakers. Doctoral dissertation in Bilingualism. Stockholm: Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Snow, R. E. (1994). Abilities in academic tasks. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (eds.), Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 337.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., & Ganschow, L. (1993). The effects of multisensory structured language instruction on native language and foreign-language aptitude skills of at-risk high-school foreign-language learners: A replication and follow-up-study. Annals of Dyslexia, 43(1), 194216.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Artzer, M., Patton, J., et al. (1998). Benefits of multisensory structured language instruction for at-risk foreign language learners: A comparison study of high school Spanish students. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 239270.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M., & Patton, J. (1997). Foreign language proficiency of at-risk and not-at-risk learners over 2 years of foreign language instruction: A follow-up study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30(1), 9298.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Ganschow, L., Fluharty, K., & Little, S. (1995). An exploratory study on the effects of Latin on the native language skills and foreign language aptitude of students with and without learning disabilities. The Classical Journal, 91(2), 165184.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Humbach, N., Patton, J., & Ganschow, L. (2011). Subcomponents of second‐language aptitude and second‐language proficiency. The Modern Language Journal, 95(2), 253273.Google Scholar
Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Pohlman, J., Skinner, S., & Artzer, M. (1992). The effects of multisensory structured language instruction on native language and foreign language aptitude skills of at-risk high school foreign language learners. Annals of Dyslexia, 42(1), 2553.Google Scholar
Speciale, G., Ellis, N. C., & Bywater, T. (2004). Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(2), 293321.Google Scholar
Spolsky, B. (1989). Conditions for Second Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, A. A., & Weaver, K. (2005). Auditory perceptual consolidation in early-onset blindness. Neuropsychologia, 43(13), 19011910.Google Scholar
Stevens, A. A., & Weaver, K. E. (2009). Functional characteristics of auditory cortex in the blind. Behavioural Brain Research, 196(1), 134138.Google Scholar
Thompson, A. S. (2013). The interface of language aptitude and multilingualism: Reconsidering the bilingual/multilingual dichotomy. The Modern Language Journal, 97(3), 685701.Google Scholar
Tomasello, R., Wennekers, T., Garagnani, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2019). Visual cortex recruitment during language processing in blind individuals is explained by Hebbian learning. Scientific Reports, 9, 3579.Google Scholar
Turker, S. (2019). Exploring the Neuroanatomical and Behavioural Correlates of Foreign Language Aptitude. Doctoral dissertation, Karl-Franzens-Universität Graz. UniPub.Google Scholar
Turker, S., Reiterer, S. M., Schneider, P., & Seither-Preisler, A. (2018). The neuroanatomical correlates of foreign language aptitude. In: Reiterer, S. (ed.), Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 119148). Springer, Cham, pp. 119–148.Google Scholar
Turker, S., Reiterer, S. M., Schneider, P., & Seither-Preisler, A. (2019). Auditory cortex morphology predicts language learning potential in children and teenagers. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 824. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00824Google Scholar
Turker, S., Reiterer, S. M., Seither-Preisler, A., & Schneider, P. (2017). “When music speaks”: Auditory Auditory cortex morphology as a neuroanatomical marker of language aptitude and musicality. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2096.Google Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M. (2016). No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2), 246258.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., Biedroń, A., & Skehan, P. (2017). Foreign language aptitude theory: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Language Teaching, 50(1), 131.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Skehan, P. (2011). A new perspective on foreign language aptitude research: Building and supporting a case for “working memory as language aptitude”. Ilha do Desterro: A Journal of English Language, Literatures in English and Cultural Studies, 60, 1544.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Skehan, P. (2021). Stages of acquisition and the P/E model of working memory: Complementary or contrasting approaches to foreign language aptitude? Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 41, 624.Google Scholar

References

Anderson, J. R. (2010). Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications, 7th ed. New York: Worth Publishing.Google Scholar
Bokander, L. (This volume, Chapter 5). Exploring the predictive validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests: A research synthesis.Google Scholar
Bokander, L., & Bylund, E. (2020). Probing the internal validity of LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning, 70(1), 1147.Google Scholar
Buffington, J., & Morgan-Short, . (2019). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language aptitude. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedron, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. L. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1962). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In Glaser, R. (ed.), Training Research and Education. Pittsburg, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 87136.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1973). Implications of aptitude test research and psycholinguistic theory for foreign language teaching. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 2, 514.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT). New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Chan, E., & Skehan, P. (2011). Developing an aptitude test based on Pienemann’s Processability Theory (Technical Report No.1). English Dept., Chinese University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Chan, E., Skehan, P., & Gong, G. (2011). Working memory, phonemic coding ability and foreign language aptitude: Potential for construction of specific language aptitude tests – the case of Cantonese. Ilho do Desterro: A Journal of English Language, Literatures and Cultural Studies, 60(1), 4573.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2019). The future of language aptitude research. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York: Routledge, pp. 317329.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. J. (2019). Cognitive language aptitude. Language Learning, 69(S1), 101126. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12322Google Scholar
Godfroid, A., & Kim, K. M. (2021). The contributions of implicit-statistical learning aptitude to implicit second language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43(3), 606634.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2013). Cognitive aptitude for second language learning and the LLAMA Language Aptitude Test. In Granena, G. & Long, M. H. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude, and Ultimate L2 Attainment.. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 105130.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency: Links between LLAMA and Hi-LAB. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(2), 313336.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2020). Implicit Language Aptitude. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019). Corrective feedback and the role of implicit sequence learning ability in L2 online performance. Language Learning, 69(S1), 127156.Google Scholar
Green, P. S. (1975). Aptitude testing: An ongoing experiment. Audio-Visual Language Journal, 12, 205210.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Sternberg, R. J., & Ehrman, M. (2002). A theory-based approach to the measurement of foreign language learning ability: The CANAL-F theory and test. Modern Language Journal, 84(3), 390405.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Hattori, H. (2006). Interpretation of English multiple wh-questions by Japanese speakers: A missing uninterpretable feature account. Second Language Research, 22(2), 269301.Google Scholar
Hughes, M., Golonka, E., Tseng, A., & Campbell, S. (This volume, Chapter 4). The High-Level Language Aptitude Test Battery (Hi-LAB): Development, Validation, and Use.Google Scholar
Kempe, V., & Brooks, P. (2016). Miniature natural language learning in L2 acquisition research. In Granena, G., Jackson, D. O., & Yilmaz, Y. (eds.), Cognitive Individual Differences in Second Language Processing and Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 4168.Google Scholar
Klein, W. (1986). Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Knowlton, B. J., Mengels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal learning system in humans. Science, 273, 13991402.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1981). Aptitude and attitude in relation to second language acquisition and learning. In Diller, K. C., K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, pp. 155175.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2015). The associations between language aptitude and second language grammar acquisition: A meta-analytic review of five decades of research. Applied Linguistics, 36(3), 385408.Google Scholar
Li, S., & Qian, J. (2021). Exploring syntactic priming as a measure of implicit language aptitude. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43(3), 132.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63(3), 530566.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In Kroll, J. F. & De Groot, A. M. B. (eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4967.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA Language Aptitude Tests. Swansea: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2011). First and Second Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and Procedural Determinants of Second Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Perruchet, P. (2021). Why is the componential construct of implicit language aptitude so difficult to capture? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43(3), 677691.Google Scholar
Petersen, C. R., & Al-Haik, A. (1976). The development of the Defense Language Aptitude Battery (DLAB). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 36, 369380.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language Processing and Second Language Development: Processability Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pimsleur, P. (1966). Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery (PLAB). New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Pimsleur, P. (1968). Language aptitude testing. In Davies, A. (ed.), Language Testing Symposium: A Psycholinguistic Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
The R Project for Statistical Computing. (2020). The R Project for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistics. https://R-Project.org/.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 6, 855863.Google Scholar
Reed, D., & Stansfield, C. (This volume, Chapter 2). The MLAT-Elementary: History, Adaptations and Current Uses.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2002). Learning conditions, aptitude complexes and SLA: A framework for research and pedagogy. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Individual Differences and Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 113133.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 4673.Google Scholar
Rogers, V., Meara, P., & Rogers, B. (This volume, Chapter 3). Testing language aptitude: LLAMA evolution and refinement.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., & Slabakova, R. (2018). The generative approach to SLA and its place in modern second language studies. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(3), 417442.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. (1996). Second Language Proficiency, Foreign Language Aptitude, and Intelligence: Quantitative and Qualitative Analyses. New York: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298 , 199209.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1988). A comparison of first and foreign language learning ability. ESOL Department, Institute of Education, UCL: Working Documents No. 8Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual Differences in Second Language Learning. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2015). Foreign language aptitude and its relationship with grammar: A critical overview. Applied Linguistics, 36(3), 367384.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2016). Foreign language aptitude, acquisitional sequences, and psycholinguistic processes. In Granena, G., Jackson, D., & Yilmaz, Y. (eds.), Cognitive Individual Differences in L2 Processing and Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1740.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2019). Language aptitude implicates language and cognitive skills. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York: Routledge, pp. 5677.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. (2012). Individual differences in L2 learning and long-term L1–L2 relationships. Language Learning, 62(Suppl. 2), 527.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. (This volume, Chapter 11). The Linguistic Coding Differences Hypothesis (LCDH) and L2 Learning: A Thirty-Year RetrospectiveGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2017). The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language. Language Learning, 67(4), 747797.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2015). The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiologically motivated theory of first and second language. In VanPatten, B. & Williams, J. (eds.), Theories in Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction, 2nd ed. London: Routledge, pp. 135158.Google Scholar
Wesche, M. B. (1981). Language aptitude measures in streaming, matching students with methods, and diagnosis of learning problems. In Diller, K. C. (ed.), Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×