Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T08:55:21.023Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - The Search for General Abilities and Basic Capacities: Theoretical Implications from the Modifiability and Complexity of Mechanisms Mediating Expert Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

The search within the social sciences for stable, invariant, and quantifiable attributes of living organisms closely parallels historic investigations in the hard sciences in search of characteristics of physical phenomena. The revolutionary advances in physics in the sixteenth through eighteenth centuries allowed scientists to develop lawful relations between characteristics of objects such as weight, size, and velocity with their subsequent “behavior” (trajectories) under specified physical conditions (for example, collisions between objects). Similarly psychology, when it emerged as an independent science in the nineteenth century, approached its own quest to uncover laws of perception and memory under the guidance of pioneering scientists such as Hermann Ebbinghaus, with comparable scientific methods. Psychology's focus was not, however, on the characteristics of physical objects, but on the search for invariant processes and attributes of individuals that could be quantified and used to predict human behavior and achievement. These same theoretical frameworks were subsequently extended to describe individual differences in ability, and finally used to predict performance in schools and other everyday settings.

In this chapter I review the search for general ability and basic capacities. I begin by briefly sketching the history of the study of individual differences and the methods pioneering investigators employed to develop theoretical conceptions of ability and capacity. Most important, I discuss the historical arguments for the existence of ability and capacity, efforts to measure these quantities, and proposals for the biological mechanisms mediating the effects of ability and capacity on performance.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abernethy, B. (1991). Visual search strategies and decision-making in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22, 189–210Google Scholar
Aikens, H. A., Thorndike, E. L., & Hubbell, E. (1902). Correlations among perceptive and associative processes. Psychological Review, 9, 374–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anastasti, A. (1988). Psychological testing (6th edition). New York: Macmillan Publishing Co
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astrand, P.-O., & Rodahl, K. (1977). Textbook of work physiology. New York: McGraw-Hill
Bernstein, N. A. (1996). Dexterity and its development. In M. L. Latash and M. T. Turvey (Eds.), Dexterity and its development (pp. 1–244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Binet, A., & Simon, Th. (1915). A method of measuring the development of the intelligence of young children. Chicago: Medical Book
Black, J. E., Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1990). Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 87, 5568–5572CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, B. S. (1985). Generalizations about talent development. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 507–549). New York: Ballantine Books
Brown, W., & Thomson, G. H. (1921). The essentials of mental measurement. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Buonomano, D. V., & Merzenich, M. M. (1998). Cortical plasticity: From synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 21, 149–186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calderwood, R., Klein, G. A., & Crandall, B. W. (1988). Time pressure, skill and move quality in chess. American Journal of Psychology, 101, 481–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N., Krampe, R. Th., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess skill acquisition. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 51–80). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 16 (pp. 1–58). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Cowley, M. (Ed.). (1959). Writers at work: The Paris Review interviews. New York: Viking Press
Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychotherapy built on myth. New York: Free Press
Deakin, J. M. (2001). What they do versus what they say they do: An assessment of practice in figure skating. In A. Papaioannou, M. Goudas, and Y. Theodorakis (Eds.), In the dawn of the new millennium: Proceedings of the International Society of Sport Psychology 10th World Congress of Sport Psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 153–155). Tessaloniki, Greece: Christodoulidi Publications
de Groot, A. (1978). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton (original work published 1946)
Drake, C., & Palmer, C. (2000). Skill acquisition in music performance: Relations between planning and temporal control. Cognition, 74, 1–32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305–307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A. (1988). Analysis of memory performance in terms of memory skill. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 4 (pp. 137–179). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance: An introduction to some of the issues. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 1–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (1998). The scientific study of expert levels of performance: General implications for optimal learning and creativity. High Ability Studies, 9, 75–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2001a). Attaining excellence through deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert performance. In M. Ferrari (Ed.), The pursuit of excellence in education (pp. 21–55). Mahwah, N. J.: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (2001b). The path to expert performance: Insights from the masters on how to improve performance by deliberate practice. In P. Thomas (Ed.), Optimizing performance in golf (pp. 1–57). Brisbane, Australia: Australian Academic Press
Ericsson, K. A. (in press). The acquisition of expert performance as problem solving: Construction and modification of mediating mechanisms through deliberate practice. In J. E. Davidson and R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Problem solving. New York: Cambridge University Press
Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 1181–1182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Patel, V. L., & Kintsch, W. (2000). How experts' adaptations to representative task demands account for the expertise effect in memory recall: Comment on Vicente and Wang (1998). Psychological Review, 107, 578–592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits in the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In K. A. Ericsson and J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 1–38). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Ericsson, K. A., & Staszewski, J. (1989). Skilled memory and expertise: Mechanisms of exceptional performance. In D. Klahr and K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 235–267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Fitts, P., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole
Galton, F., Sir (1869/1979). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Julian Friedman Publishers
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look-ahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from grand-master-level chess. Psychological Science, 7, 52–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, E. B. (1937). Practice effects on various types of standard tests. American Journal of Psychology, 49, 67–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helsen, W. F., Starkes, J. L., & Hodges, N. J. (1998). Team sports and the theory of deliberate practice. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 12–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodges, N. J., & Starkes, J. L. (1996). Wrestling with the nature of expertise: A sport specific test of Ericsson, Krampe and Tesch-Römer's (1993) theory of “Deliberate Practice,”International Journal of Sport Psychology, 27, 400–424Google Scholar
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karpov, A. (1995). Grandmaster musings. Chess Life, November, pp. 32–33
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q., (1998). Brain plasticity and behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 43–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koltanowski, G. (1985). In the dark. Coraopolis, PA: Chess Enterprises
Krampe, R. Th., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehman, H. C. (1953). Age and achievement. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1993). Sight-reading ability of expert pianists in the context of piano accompanying. Psychomusicology, 12(2), 182–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1995). Expert pianists' mental representation of memorized music. Poster presented at the 36th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Los Angeles, CA, November 10–12
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Music performance without preparation: Structure and acquisition of expert sight-reading. Psychomusicology, 15, 1–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson K. A. (1997). Expert pianists' mental representations: Evidence from successful adaptation to unexpected performance demands. Proceedings of the Third Triennial ESCOM Conference (pp. 165–169). Uppsala, Sweden: SLU Service/Reproenheten
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson K. A. (1998). The historical development of domains of expertise: Performance standards and innovations in music. In A. Steptoe (Ed.), Genius and the mind (pp. 67–94). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits of our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, J. (1999). Measurement in psychology: Critical history of a methodological concept. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.). (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Plimpton, G. (Ed.). (1977). Writers at work: The Paris Review interviews. New York: Penguin Books
Saariluoma, P. (1991). Aspects of skilled imagery in blindfold chess. Acta Psychologica, 77, 65–89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345–371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlaug, G., Janke, L., Huang, Y., Staiger, J. F., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). Increased corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologica, 33, 1047–1055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113–120Google Scholar
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394–403Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. A. (1984). Experimental studies in music reading: A review. Music Perception, 22, 222–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloboda, J. A. (1996). The acquisition of musical performance expertise: Deconstructing the “talent” account of individual differences in musical expressivity. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 107–126). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Sloboda, J. A., Clarke, E. F., Parncutt, R., & Raekallio, M. (1998). Determinants of finger choice in piano sight-reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 185–203Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. A., Davidson, J. W., Howe, M. J. A., & Moore, D. G. (1996). The role of practice in the development of performing musicians. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 287–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of ‘intelligence and the principles of cognition. London: Macmillan
Spearman, C. (1937). Psychology down the ages, Vol. II. London: Macmillan
Stael von Holstein, C.-A. S., (1972). Probabilistic forecasting: An experiment related to the stock market. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 139–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starkes, J. L., Deakin, J., Allard, F., Hodges, N. J., & Hayes, A. (1996). Deliberate practice in sports: What is it anyway? In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 81–106). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Williams, A. M., David, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual perception and action in sport. New York: Routledge
Wissler, C. (1901). The correlation of mental physical tests. Psychological Review Monographs, 3(6), 1–62 (Whole No. 16)Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S., & Thorndike, E. L. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions: III. Functions involving attention, observation and discrimination. Psychological Review, 8, 553–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, R. M. (1970). Mind, brain and adaptation in the nineteenth century. Oxford, UK; Clarendon Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×