Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T16:15:03.523Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Morphologically Assigned Accent and an Initial Three-Syllable Window in Ese’eja

from Part III - Case Studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2018

Rob Goedemans
Affiliation:
Universiteit Leiden
Jeffrey Heinz
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University, State University of New York
Harry van der Hulst
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Rolle and Vuillermet argue that Ese’eja demonstrates an unusual initial three-syllable window within which primary prominence must fall, a typologically rare type. The authors show that the position of prominence depends on syllable count and the type of morphologically assigned accent. They posit four types of this morphological accent: inherent transitive accent, dominant indexical accent, recessive accent with one set of tense/mood suffixes, and rightmost-preserving accent with another. Further, tense/mood suffixes trigger the creation of iterative trochaic or iambic feet, which the authors capture using cophonology theory employing common OT constraints. The authors posit that iterative footing occurs with a leftmost constraint, resulting in primary accent falling on the first, second, or third syllable, which is realized with primary prominence. Ese’eja constitutes a true ‘count system’ challenging the Primary Accent First model. Finally, they argue that when morphological accent in Ese’eja is assigned outside the metrical window, the position of primary prominence falls on a rhythmically dependent position, termed ‘rhythmic repair’.
Type
Chapter
Information
The Study of Word Stress and Accent
Theories, Methods and Data
, pp. 361 - 386
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anttila, A. (2002). Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20(1): 142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, R. (2013). The uniqueness of metrical structure: Rhythmic phonotactics in Huariapano. Phonology 30: 355398.Google Scholar
Bickmore, L. and Broadwell, G. A.. (1998). High tone docking in Sierra Juárez Zapotec. International Journal of American Linguistics 64(1): 3767.Google Scholar
Caballero, G. (2011). Morphologically conditioned stress assignment in Choguita Rarámuri (Tarahumara). Linguistics 49(4): 749790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, W. (1986). El idioma Chiriguano: Gramática, textos, vocabulario. Madrid: Ediciones Cultura Hispánica, Instituto de Cooperación Iberoamericana.Google Scholar
Goedemans, R. and van der Hulst, H. (2013). Rhythm types. In Dryer, M. S. and Haspelmath, M. (eds.) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. wals.info/chapter/17.Google Scholar
Goedemans, R. and van der Hulst, H.(2014). The separation of accent and rhythm: Evidence from StressTyp. In van der Hulst, H. (ed.) Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge University Press, 119146.Google Scholar
González, C. (2016). Tipología de los sistemas métricos de veinticinco lenguas pano. Amerindia 39(1): 129172.Google Scholar
Guillaume, A. (2008a). A Grammar of Cavineña. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Guillaume, A. (2008b). Le Cavineña et les langues tacana comme exemples de systèmes à transitivité très fortement grammaticalisée. Paper presented at the Atelier de Morphosyntaxe, Lyon, Laboratoire Dynamique Du Langage, November.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1995). Metrical Stress Theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hulst, H. van der. (1984). Syllable Structure and Stress in Dutch. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Hulst, H. van der. (1996). Separating primary accent and secondary accent. In Goedemans, R., van der Hulst, H., and Visch, E. (eds.) Stress Patterns of the World. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics, 126.Google Scholar
Hulst, H. van der. (2012). Deconstructing stress. Lingua 122: 14941521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, B. (2011). The iambic-trochaic law. In van Oostendorp, M., Ewen, C., Hume, B., and Rice, K. (eds.), A Companion to Phonology. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, 10521077.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. (2009). How (not) to do phonological typology: The case of pitch-accent. Language Sciences 31: 213238.Google Scholar
Inkelas, S. (1998). The theoretical status of morphologically conditioned phonology: A case study from dominance. In Booij, G. and Marle, J. (eds.) Yearbook of Morphology 1997. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 121155.Google Scholar
Inkelas, S. and Zoll, C.. (2007). Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned phonology. Linguistics 45: 133171.Google Scholar
Kager, R. (1999). Optimality Theory. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kager, R. (2012). Stress in windows: Language typology and factorial typology. Lingua 122: 14541493.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S. (2015). The phonology of Japanese accent. In Kubozono, H. (ed.) The Handbook of Japanese Language and Linguistics: Phonetics and Phonology. Berlin and Boston: Mouton de Gruyter, 445492Google Scholar
Kawahara, S. and Kao, S.. (2012). The productivity of a root-initial accenting suffix, [-zu]: Judgement studies. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 30(3): 837857.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S. and Wolf, M.. (2010). On the existence of root-initial-accenting suffixes: An elicitation study of Japanese [-zu]. Linguistics 48: 837864.Google Scholar
Key, M. R. (1968). Comparative Tacanan Phonology, with Cavineña Phonology and Notes on Pano-Tacanan Relationships. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Key, M. R. and Wyma, R.. (1964). Analyzing stress in Chama (Tacanan). Chapman College Research Bulletin 1(1): 2837.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1984). A compositional approach to Vedic word accent. In Joshi, S. D. (ed.) Amrtadhara: Prof R. N. Dandekar felicitation volume. Delhi: Ajanta, 201210.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. and Halle, M.. (1977). Towards a reconstruction of the Indo-European accent. In Hyman, L. (ed.) Studies in Stress and Accent. Los Angeles: University of Southern California, 209238.Google Scholar
McCawley, J. D. (1968). The Phonological Component of a Grammar of Japanese. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Michael, L., Chang, W., and Stark, T.. (2014). Exploring phonological areality in the circum-Andean region using a naive bayes classifier. Language Dynamics and Change 4(1): 2786.Google Scholar
Orgun, C. O. (1996). Sign-based morphology and phonology: With special attention to Optimality Theory. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Palancar, E. L. and Léonard, J. L.. (2016). Tone and Inflection. Berlin and Boston: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pitman, D. and Pitman, M.. (1978). Araona syllable and stress. In Wiesemann, U. (ed.) Working Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics 1972–1976. Riberalta: Summer Institute of Linguistics, 117.Google Scholar
Poser, W. (1984). The phonetics and phonology of tone and intonation in Japanese. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Prince, A. and Smolenksy, P.. (1993). Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science Technical Report 2.Google Scholar
Rolle, Nicholas. (2013). Ese Ejja accent. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report 2013, 268280.Google Scholar
Sakel, J. (2004). A Grammar of Mosetén. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Tallman, A. J. (2014). The prosody of split and glued verb constructions in Chácobo (Pano). Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 40: 495522.Google Scholar
Tejada, L. (2012). Tone gestures and constraint interaction in Sierra Juarez Zapotec. PhD dissertation, University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, P. (2003). Transitivity in Shipibo-Konibo grammar. PhD dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, P. (2010). Multi-verb predicates and transitivity harmony in Shipibo-Konibo. In Aikhenvald, A. Y. and Muysken, P. C. (eds.) Multi-verb Constructions: A View from the Americas. Leiden: Brill, 139159.Google Scholar
Vuillermet, M. (2006). L’ese eja de Bolivie (langue tacana): Esquisse phonologique et phonétique. Master Thesis, Université Lumière Lyon 2.Google Scholar
Vuillermet, M. (2008). La transitivité en ese ejja. Paper presented at the Atelier de Morphosyntaxe, Lyon, Laboratoire Dynamique du Langage, November.Google Scholar
Vuillermet, M. (2012). A grammar of Ese Ejja, a Takanan language of the Bolivian Amazon. PhD dissertation, Université Lumière Lyon 2. www.marinevuillermet.com/%20these/complete-thesis/.Google Scholar
Wetzels, L. and Meira, S.. (2010). A survey of South American stress systems. In van der Hulst, H., Goedemans, R., and van Zanten, E. (eds.) A Survey of Word Accentual Patterns in the Languages of the World. Berlin and New York: Walter de Gruyter, 313380.Google Scholar
Wynen, D. van and de Garrard, M. van Wynen, . (1962). Fonemas Tacana y modelos de acentuacion. Notas linguisticas de Bolivia No. 6. Cochabamba, Bolivia: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×