Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T00:21:32.127Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does all teaching rest on evolved traits?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Laura Chouinard-Thuly
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec H3A 1B1, Canada. laura.chouinard-thuly@mail.mcgill.casimon.reader@mcgill.cahttp://biology.mcgill.ca/faculty/reader/
Simon M. Reader
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec H3A 1B1, Canada. laura.chouinard-thuly@mail.mcgill.casimon.reader@mcgill.cahttp://biology.mcgill.ca/faculty/reader/ Department of Biology and Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, 3508 TB Utrecht, The Netherlands.

Abstract

Classification schemes are useful when they elucidate common underlying mechanisms, bring together diverse examples, or illustrate gaps in knowledge for empirical investigation. Kline's scheme merges different approaches, but is orthogonal to existing schemes and overemphasizes evolved specializations, potentially at the detriment of clarifying teaching processes. Focus on underlying mechanisms, what is learned, and consequences for information transfer may provide additional utility.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? The Quarterly Review of Biology 67(2):151–74. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2831436 Google Scholar
Coolen, I., van Bergen, Y., Day, R. L. & Laland, K. N. (2003) Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270:2413–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4):148–53.Google Scholar
Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J. & Wagner, R. H. (2004) Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305:487–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, E. H., Avarguès-Weber, A., Chittka, L. & Leadbeater, E. (2013) Learning by observation emerges from simple associations in an insect model. Current Biology 23:727–30.Google Scholar
de Kort, S. R. & Clayton, N. S. (2006) An evolutionary perspective on caching by corvids. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 273:417–23.Google Scholar
Feldon, D. F., Peugh, J., Timmerman, B. E., Maher, M. A., Hurst, M., Strickland, D., Gilmore, J. A. & Stiegelmeyer, C. (2011) Graduate students' teaching experiences improve their methodological research skills. Science 333:1037–39.Google Scholar
Franks, N. & Richardson, T. (2006) Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature 439(7073):153.Google Scholar
Grüter, C. & Leadbeater, E. (2014) Insights from insects about adaptive social information use. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 29:177–84.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994) Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 69(2):207231.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2012a) Grist and mills: On the cultural origins of cultural learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367:2181–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. M. (2012b) What's social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology 126:193202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppitt, W. J. E., Brown, G. R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M. M. & Laland, K. N. (2008) Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23(9):486–93. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.05.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleindorfer, S., Hoi, H., Evans, C., Mahr, K., Robertson, J., Hauber, M. E. & Colombelli-Négrel, D. (2014) The cost of teaching embryos in superb fairy-wrens. Behavioral Ecology 25:1131–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Komdeur, J. (1996) Influence of helping and breeding experience on reproductive performance in the Seychelles warbler: A translocation experiment. Behavioral Ecology 7:326–33.Google Scholar
Lindeyer, C. M., Meaney, M. J. & Reader, S. M. (2013) Early maternal care predicts reliance on social learning about food in adult rats. Developmental Psychobiology 55:168–75.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. (2014) Experiential effects on mirror systems and social learning: Implications for social intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37(2):217–18.Google Scholar
Richardson, T. O., Houston, A. I. & Franks, N. R. (2007) Teaching with evaluation in ants. Current Biology 17:1520–26.Google Scholar
Seppänen, J.-T., Forsman, J. T., Mönkkönen, M. & Thomson, R. L. (2007) Social information use is a process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. Ecology 88:1622–33.Google Scholar
Thornton, A. & Raihani, N. (2008) The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour 75(6):1823–36. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar