Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2xdlg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-17T12:45:18.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The social side of innovation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Bruce Rawlings
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX78712. bruce.rawlings@utexas.edu chlegare@gmail.com https://www.brucerawlings.com/ http://www.cristinelegare.com/
Cristine H. Legare
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX78712. bruce.rawlings@utexas.edu chlegare@gmail.com https://www.brucerawlings.com/ http://www.cristinelegare.com/

Abstract

Innovation is fundamental to cumulative culture, allowing progressive modification of existing technology. The authors define innovation as an asocial process, uninfluenced by social information. We argue that innovation is inherently social – innovation is frequently the product of modifying others' outputs, and successful innovations are acquired by others. Research should target examination of the cognitive underpinnings of socially-mediated innovations.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beck, S. R., Williams, C., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A. & Chappell, J. (2016) Individual differences in children's innovative problem-solving are not predicted by divergent thinking or executive functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1690):20150190. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, K., Kendal, R. L. & Flynn, E. G. (2015) Imitate or innovate? Children's innovation is influenced by the efficacy of observed behaviour. Cognition 142:322–32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, K., Kendal, R. L. & Flynn, E. G. (2016) Eureka!: What is innovation, how does it develop, and who does it? Child Development 87(5):1505–19. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J. & Beck, S. R. (2019) Is tool modification more difficult than innovation? Cognitive Development 52:100811. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legare, C. H. & Nielsen, M. (2015) Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19(11):688–99. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M. & Henrich, J. (2016) Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1690):20150192. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, G., Bastian, M. L. & van Schaik, C. (2007) Animal innovation defined and operationalized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30:393437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, C. & Flynn, E. (2016) Learning by diffusion: Using diffusion experiments and social network analysis to understand the dynamics of cultural evolution. In: Shared representations, eds. Obhi, S. S. & Cross, E. S., pp. 503–24. Cambridge University Press. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107279353.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar