We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
The vast majority of existing research on outcomes following concussion is androcentric with women notably understudied in all settings where concussion commonly occurs, including sport, military, and civilian domains. Clinical care for concussed women is, by extension, far less evidenced-based as it is largely extrapolated from male-dominated studies. We therefore sought to center concussion outcomes of women in big data sources in this investigation to capitalize on both sample size and breadth of setting in which concussion occurs.
Participants and Methods:
We identified all studies with publicly released data as of 4/7/21 that included both male and female adults, enough information to determine severity of injury consistent with concussion/mild traumatic brain injury (TBI), a measure of postconcussive symptoms (PCS), and objective measures of neurocognitive functioning from the Federal Interagency Traumatic Brain Injury Research (FITBIR) Informatics System. FITBIR is a collaborative effort of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and the Department of Defense (DoD), developed to share data across the entire TBI research community. This resulted in inclusion of six studies with a total of 9370 participants, 32% female. PCS data was drawn from the Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory (NSI), the Rivermead Post-Concussion Symptoms Questionnaire (RPSQ), and the Sport Concussion Assessment Tool - 3rd Edition (SCAT 3), specifically the 22-item participant symptom evaluation rated on a 7-point scale. The questionnaires were harmonized and the following symptom domains emerged: Somatic, Cognitive, and Affective. Data were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models.
Results:
We found a small to medium sized significant effect of sex, with women reporting higher symptoms overall. Mean symptom endorsement scores were higher overall on the NSI in comparison to the RPSQ and SCAT (ps < .001). Follow-up analyses revealed sex differences were largest for cognitive symptoms, followed by somatic symptoms, then affective symptoms. We also found significant main effects of population (military>sport/civilian, p = .003) and sex (women>males, p < .001) on the overall composite, as well as a significant population-by-sex interaction such that female service members/veterans endorsed the highest rates of symptoms (p < .001). Similar patterns of significance and effect sizes were observed for the somatic and affective composites. Cognitive symptom composites showed a similar pattern, but with smaller effect sizes overall. Racial and ethnic diversity was also limited in the sample.
Conclusions:
In one of the largest samples of women to date, we found a small to medium effect of sex on symptom reporting such that women reported higher levels of postconcussive symptoms than males. Notably, however, the women in military/Veteran samples endorsed the highest levels of symptoms. Despite using a large publicly available dataset to maximize the representation of women, the current sample was still predominantly male and racial and ethnic diversity among the sample was not consistent with expected broader population demographics. Dramatically more concerted efforts need to be made to engage women in all spheres of concussion research (military, civilian, and sport). Strategies to be more inclusive in concussion research will be highlighted.
Stephenson (2000) suggested that acculturation is a phenomenon that immigrants and refugees ubiquitously experience. The level of acculturation is impacted by a person’s choice to allow how much of their cultural traits they decide to keep while adapting to the dominant society cultural traits. Depending on what immigrants find to be important or unimportant, it can influence future generations (i.e., their children) in how they will be developed and adapt into a dominant society. Hispanic-Americans are individuals that were born and reside in the United States and have a family background that extends to one of the Spanish speaking countries in Latin America or Spain. The typical language spoken by Hispanic families other than English is Spanish. It has been reported that Hispanics that are capable of speaking English may be afforded better and greater opportunities to resources. Research shows that a person level of acculturation can influence their cognition. In fact, in one study using a Mexican-American sample that was divided into two groups: high and low. Researchers found that highly acculturated Mexican-Americans outperformed lower acculturated Mexican-Americans on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. We evaluated the influence of acculturation in Hispanic-Americans neuropsychological test performance. It was predicted that highly acculturated Hispanic-Americans to American culture would demonstrate better cognitive abilities compared to lower acculturated Hispanic-Americans.
Participants and Methods:
The present study sample consisted of 75 neurologically and psychologically healthy Hispanic-American undergraduate students with a mean age of 19.44 (SD = 1.37). Participants were divided into two acculturation groups: high (n = 39) and low (n = 36). In addition, all the participants completed a comprehensive neuropsychological battery and background questionnaire in English. The Acculturation Rating Scale for Hispanic/Latino Americans is a 20-item scale that was utilized to create our acculturation groups. ANOVAs were used to evaluate cognitive differences between our acculturation groups.
Results:
Results revealed that the highly acculturated group outperformed the lower acculturated group on the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition vocabulary task and the Boston Naming Test, p’s<.05, n.p’s2=.06. Furthermore, results revealed that the lower acculturated group outperformed the highly acculturated group on the Trail Making Test part A and B, and Comalli Stroop part A, p’s<.05, np’s2=.06-.07.
Conclusions:
As expected, the highly acculturated group demonstrated better language abilities compared to the lower acculturated group. However, in the opposite direction, the lower acculturated group outperformed the highly acculturated group on several speed attention tasks and one executive functioning task. A possible explanation why the highly acculturated participants demonstrated better language abilities may be attributed that their dominant language is English or they only spoke English. Meanwhile, the opposite could be said for lower acculturated participants that English was not their dominant language or they were bilingual speakers, for that reason they demonstrated better processing speed and executive functioning abilities. Research shows that monolinguals demonstrate better language abilities compared to bilinguals, but the opposite is found on processing speed and executive functioning tasks. Future research should investigate the relationship between bilingualism and acculturation in neuropsychological testing performance of Hispanic-Americans.
Our objective is to investigate whether handedness is associated with performance on verbal and visual neuropsychological tests in people with severe mental disorders. A recent study, applying a continuous scale of hand preference, reports that handedness is not associated with test performance in people with schizophrenia disorders. Conversely, in a recent large meta-analysis where handedness was applied as a dichotomous variable, right-handers had better performance in spatial ability (but not verbal ability) compared to left-handers, irrespective of gender or health status. We hypothesize that a dichotomous classification of handedness will reveal an advantage of right-handedness on tests of visuospatial functions—but not verbal functions—in people with severe mental disorders. We expect that gender will not be associated with the neuropsychological test results.
Participants and Methods:
Data from a sample of 385 patients with severe mental disorders, mainly within the schizophrenia spectrum, were analyzed. All participants had Norwegian as their first language. Their mean age was 24.8 years (SD=6.2) and 153 (39.7%) were women. Handedness was evaluated by observation of preferred hand in writing and drawing during neuropsychological assessment. Chi-square tests were used to compare proportions of cases with reported frequencies of handedness in the general population and comparable clinical samples. Raw scores on Semantic Fluency and Line Orientation from the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U tests, and possible effects of gender with two-way ANOVA. Frequencies of low scores were analyzed using frequency analyses.
Results:
Overall prevalence of left-handers was 10.4% compared to 10.6% in the general population (χ2=.018, p=.893). Observed prevalence for women was 9.2% compared to expected prevalence of 9.5% (χ2=.026, p=.873) and for men 10.7% and 11.6%, respectively (χ2=.039, p=.844). There was a significant difference in Line Orientation scores (Mdright-handers=18, Mdleft-handers=17; U=5268.0, p=.013) but not Semantic Fluency scores (Mdright-handers=17, Mdleft-handers=18.5; U=7568.5, p=.315). Right-handed men had higher scores on Line Orientation but there was no handedness by gender interaction (F(1)=1.69, p=.194). For Semantic Fluency, left-handed men had higher scores and a gender by handedness interaction was found (F(1)=7.21, p=.008). Using scores corresponding to <5th percentile, 15% of left-handers and 8% of right-handers had scores <5th percentile on Line Orientation, as opposed to 15% and 14% on Semantic Fluency.
Conclusions:
Right-handers had significantly better performance on a test measuring visuospatial function, irrespective of gender. Left-handers had about twice the number of scores in the impaired range (i.e., <5th percentile) compared to right-handers. Left-handed men had better performance on a test of verbal functions, which was unexpected. A recent study reported no right-hand associated advantage on visuospatial tests in people with schizophrenia disorders when measuring handedness on a continuous scale. This suggests that the classification of handedness as either a dichotomous or as a continuous variable is important in studies of handedness and cognitive functions.
Migraine is one of the leading causes of disability worldwide and a recognized contributor to health disparities with public health implications. Although migraine is a highly prevalent neurological condition, research on the cognitive manifestations of migraines is inconsistent. Studies have confirmed neurocognitive compromise during the presence of a migraine attack, with it’s onset and frequency being associated with greater cognitive decline. Research on the cognitive implications of migraines in underserved communities is scarce. The American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention Study (2015) found that the prevalence of chronic migraines was the highest amongst Hispanic females compared to White females. Latina/os are 50% less likely to receive a migraine diagnosis and adequate headache medication when compared to non-Hispanic White patients. Latina women were more likely to report somatic symptoms than White and African American participants (F=8.96; p>0.001) (Liefland et al., 2014). Somatoform disorders are often diagnosed amongst Latina/os to account for medical unexplained symptoms, and if misdiagnosed can be stigmatizing and detrimental to treatment. We illustrate the critical role that neuropsychologists play by utilizing Socially Responsible Neuropsychology (SRN), a theoretical framework that promotes equitable and precise neuropsychological care to reduce misdiagnosis, elucidate cognitive implications, and address the complex medical needs of Latina/o/ patients. Given the limited literature on migraines and neurocognitive functioning, our objective is to present two case studies to illustrate the neuropsychological implications among bilingual Latina/o with chronic migraines.
Participants and Methods:
Two highly educated Latina/o women, ages 39 and 41 years old, with chronic migraines, cognitive decline, diagnoses and a history of somatization symptoms. The onset of symptoms was gradual, worsening in intensity and frequency, along with notable motor symptoms (e.g., paralysis, weakness, numbness, bilateral tremors), photophobia, and phonophobia. Their cognitive complaints were conceptualized as part of a somatoform presentation by their providers.
Results:
The SRN model guided clinical decision-making to establish reliable normative anchors to identify relative impairment compared to premorbid estimates. Testing was completed in English after establishing language dominance via English and Spanish measures of verbal fluency. Cognitive profiles identified declines in attention, processing speed, language, perceptual reasoning, visual memory, executive functioning, motor functioning, and notable decline in their functioning over several years. The neuropsychological profile discounted the presence of a somatoform disorder. One case was diagnosed with an Unspecified Mild Neurocognitive Disorder, while the other case met criteria for a Major Neurocognitive Disorder due to Multiple Etiologies (i.e., vascular contribution, migraines, history of other contributions- choking episode).
Conclusions:
Given the decline in each profile, it was hypothesized that the patients’ utilization of compensatory strategies and higher education may have masked the onset of symptoms. These complex cases highlight the need for comprehensive neuropsychological evaluations that are culturally and linguistically responsive to boost the sensitivity of accurate diagnosis. The ability to objectively capture neurocognitive decline offers a unique opportunity to enhance treatment, which would have otherwise remained undetected and untreated. The SRN model enhanced diagnostic considerations, treatment planning, and allowed for advocacy strategies to improve the quality of life, and access to culturally/linguistically appropriate resources.
The psychometric properties of the Salthouse Listening Span (SLS; Salthouse & Babcock, 1991) task are relatively unknown.
Previous research has demonstrated that SLS performance is positively associated with processing speed and vocabulary (Salthouse, 2005). Further research has documented that SLS performance is useful in differentiating attention deficit/hyperactive disorder from other clinical conditions (Nikolas, Marshall, & Hoelzle, 2019). While the SLS task is purported to measure working memory, relatively little is known about how the task is related to frequently administered neuropsychological measures. Furthermore, it remains unclear whether emotional functioning may affect task performance. The current study investigates associations between frequently administered tasks and the SLS as well as the impact of anxiety and depression on SLS performance.
Participants and Methods:
A battery of neuropsychological tasks and self-report measures was administered to undergraduates [N=161, 75.2% female, Mage=19 (1.06), MGPA=3.5(.35)]. Participant exclusion based on failed performance validity task, non-native English speaking, and/or task incompletion resulted in a final sample of N=131. Participants completed the SLS, a task where one answers questions about sentences read aloud to them, while simultaneously attempting to remember the final word from sentences. SLS performance was quantified two ways: (1) longest span score (SLS-LSS) and (2) total words recalled correctly (SLS-WRC). Anxiety and depression were measured via the Beck Anxiety Index (BAI) and the Beck Depression Index (BDI). Two groups were derived based on participant BAI and BDI responses: low to mild emotional distress (N=99, scores of 0-15 on BAI, BDI, or both) and moderate to severe emotional distress (N=33, scores of 16-63 on the BDI, BAI, or both). Correlations were conducted between the SLS and WAIS-IV: digit span, arithmetic, coding, and symbol search, DKEFS: verbal fluency, and WTAR. A one-way ANOVA was run to examine potential differences in performance on the SLS based on levels of emotional distress.
Results:
The SLS-LSS had negligible correlations with verbal fluency, coding, or symbol search performances (r<0.1). SLS-LSS demonstrated a small to medium positive correlation with arithmetic [r(130)=0.17, p=.06], digit span [r(130)=0.27, p=.002], and WTAR [r(130)=.27, p=.002]. SLS-WRC did not demonstrate meaningful correlations with any cognitive domain. Overall, the presence of moderate anxiety and/or moderate depression did not significantly affect performance on SLS- LSS [F(1, 130)= 1.5, p=0.22] or SLS-WRC [F(1, 132)=0.55, p=0.46].
Conclusions:
The SLS is a promising cognitive task with little research investigating its psychometric properties. Overall, minimal correlations were observed with tasks quantifying executive functioning, verbal abilities, and processing speed. Lack of strong correlations indicate that more research should be conducted to fully understand what this task is measuring. Moreover, the SLS-WRC score did not appear to have significant correlations across domains, indicating that the SLS-LSS may be more strongly related to working memory and general intelligence. Encouragingly, emotional functioning did not appear to impact performance on this task. While the SLS appears to have some relation to IQ, more research should be conducted to determine what this task measures and what variables may affect task performance.
To examine relationships between history of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), neuropsychological measures of executive function, and lifetime history of criminal justice (CJ) involvement among combat-exposed Veterans and Service Members (V/SM).
Participants and Methods:
Participants were combat-exposed V/SM who completed a baseline assessment for the multicenter Long-term Impact of Military-Relevant Brain Injury Consortium - Chronic Effects of Neurotrauma Consortium study (N=1,341) and had adequate engagement/symptom reporting on measures of performance and symptom validity (i.e., Medical Symptom Validity Test and Mild Brain Injury Atypical Symptoms Scale). Neuropsychological battery included the Trail Making Test (A and B), Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) Digit Span subtest, and the National Institute of Health (NIH) Toolbox Flanker subtest. Lifetime history of brain injury, criminal justice involvement, and demographics were collected. Participants were 87% male, 72% white, with a mean age of 40 years (SD=9.67). Eighty-one percent had at least some college education. Nineteen percent were active duty. Eighty percent of Veterans and 86% of Service Members reported a history of >1 mTBI, and of these 31% and 47% respectively experienced 3+ mTBIs.
Results:
Three groups were composed based on level of involvement with the CJ system: 1.) No history of arrests or incarcerations (3+ mTBIs: 64%), 2.) A lifetime history of arrest but no felony incarceration (3+ mTBIs: 34%), and 3.) A lifetime history of felony incarceration (3+ mTBIs: 2%). Ordinal regression analyses revealed that performance on a working memory task (Digit Span; b= 0.024, p= .041; OR= 1.024) was significantly associated with increased CJ involvement after adjusting for age, education, service status, and mTBIs. Performance on measures of processing speed (Trails A), set-shifting (Trails B), and inhibition (Flanker) were not significantly associated with CJ involvement. Number of mTBIs was significantly and positively associated with level of CJ involvement in all four models; Digit Span (p= .016), Trails A (p= .016), Trails B (p= .020), and Flanker (p= .008).
Conclusions:
Performance on most measures of executive functioning was not significantly associated with CJ involvement in this large, representative sample of V/SM who served in combat. Although performance on a working memory task was significantly associated with CJ involvement, the size of the effect was small and the association was in the opposite direction as expected. Number of mTBIs was significantly associated with level of CJ involvement, indicating that sustaining multiple mTBI may be linked to greater risk of CJ involvement. These findings suggest that social and psychological factors beyond executive dysfunction may better explain the relationship between history mTBIs and CJ involvement. Some aspects of military service and veteran status, such as interdisciplinary treatment for brain injury and physical, mental, and psychosocial health needs, may be protective against previously identified risk factors for arrest (e.g., deficits in executive functioning). Contextualizing mTBI within the larger behavioral health profile of V/SM, with emphasis placed on intervention for related co-morbidities, may reduce the impact of previous arrest on wellbeing and/or reduce the risk of future CJ involvement.
The locus coeruleus (LC) innervates the cerebrovasculature and plays a crucial role in optimal regulation of cerebral blood flow. However, no human studies to date have examined links between these systems with widely available neuroimaging methods. We quantified associations between LC structural integrity and regional cortical perfusion and probed whether varying levels of plasma Alzheimer’s disease (AD) biomarkers (Aß42/40 ratio and ptau181) moderated these relationships.
Participants and Methods:
64 dementia-free community-dwelling older adults (ages 55-87) recruited across two studies underwent structural and functional neuroimaging on the same MRI scanner. 3D-pCASL MRI measured regional cerebral blood flow in limbic and frontal cortical regions, while T1-FSE MRI quantified rostral LC-MRI contrast, a well-established proxy measure of LC structural integrity. A subset of participants underwent fasting blood draw to measure plasma AD biomarker concentrations (Aß42/40 ratio and ptau181). Multiple linear regression models examined associations between perfusion and LC integrity, with rostral LC-MRI contrast as predictor, regional CBF as outcome, and age and study as covariates. Moderation analyses included additional terms for plasma AD biomarker concentration and plasma x LC interaction.
Results:
Greater rostral LC-MRI contrast was linked to lower regional perfusion in limbic regions, such as the amygdala (ß = -0.25, p = 0.049) and entorhinal cortex (ß = -0.20, p = 0.042), but was linked to higher regional perfusion in frontal cortical regions, such as the lateral (ß = 0.28, p = 0.003) and medial (ß = 0.24, p = 0.05) orbitofrontal (OFC) cortices. Plasma amyloid levels moderated the relationship between rostral LC and amygdala CBF (Aß42/40 ratio x rostral LC interaction term ß = -0.31, p = 0.021), such that as plasma Aß42/40 ratio decreased (i.e., greater pathology), the strength of the negative relationship between rostral LC integrity and amygdala perfusion decreased. Plasma ptau181levels moderated the relationship between rostral LC and entorhinal CBF (ptau181 x rostral LC interaction term ß = 0.64, p = 0.001), such that as ptau181 increased (i.e., greater pathology), the strength of the negative relationship between rostral LC integrity and entorhinal perfusion decreased. For frontal cortical regions, ptau181 levels moderated the relationship between rostral LC and lateral OFC perfusion (ptau181 x rostral LC interaction term ß = -0.54, p = .004), as well as between rostral LC and medial OFC perfusion (ptau181 x rostral LC interaction term ß = -0.53, p = .005), such that as ptau181 increased (i.e., greater pathology), the strength of the positive relationship between rostral LC integrity and frontal perfusion decreased.
Conclusions:
LC integrity is linked to regional cortical perfusion in non-demented older adults, and these relationships are moderated by plasma AD biomarker concentrations. Variable directionality of the associations between the LC and frontal versus limbic perfusion, as well as the differential moderating effects of plasma AD biomarkers, may signify a compensatory mechanism and a shifting pattern of hyperemia in the presence of aggregating AD pathology. Linking LC integrity and cerebrovascular regulation may represent an important understudied pathway of dementia risk and may help to bridge competing theories of dementia progression in preclinical AD studies.
The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM) is a performance validity test (PVT) that aims to assess whether participants are giving adequate effort to perform well on tasks of memory performance (Tombaugh, 1996). Other PVTs, specifically the Forced Choice Recognition Trial in the California Verbal Learning Test, have shown that even single errors may indicate invalid performance (Erdodi et al., 2018). Finally, youth are often understudied in the PVT literature, and athletes are at increased risk of invalid performance on baseline testing due to many wanting to return to play following concussion (Erdal, 2012). Therefore, the objective of the current study is to examine whether single errors on TOMM Trial 1 are indicative of lower, and possibly invalid, cognitive performance in a youth sample, given that cognitive performance declines with even small decreases in effort (Green, 2007).
Participants and Methods:
Healthy youth athletes (n=174) aged 8-16 years (M=12.07) completed the TOMM as well as other neuropsychological measures during baseline neuropsychological evaluation in a clinical research program for sports concussion. Independent samples t-tests compared youth athletes who scored 49 points on the TOMM (n=28) to youth athletes who scored a perfect 50 (n=50) across several groupings of neuropsychological measures. Participants who scored less than 49 or who didn’t complete the TOMM were excluded from the analyses.
Results:
Participants scoring 50/50 on TOMM Trial 1 scored significantly higher on Stroop Color Naming task (p=0.036), Verbal Learning Delayed task from the second edition of the Wide Range Assessment of Learning and Memory (WRAML-2, p=0.018), and Letter Number Sequencing task from the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV, p=0.025), relative to participants scoring 49/50. Though not statistically significant, results also showed a trend toward participants scoring 50/50 scoring higher on nearly every test in the battery.
Conclusions:
Participants with a single error on TOMM Trial 1, as compared to participants with a perfect score, performed significantly worse on a processing speed task, a verbal learning task, and a working memory task as part of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery. The single-error group also trended toward scoring lower on nearly all of the remaining attention, processing speed, perceptual ability, memory, and executive functioning tasks in the battery. The results could lead to a more liberal interpretation of TOMM scores, given that the trend towards lower performance may be due to putting forth significantly less effort. These results point to the need for a similar comparison of the TOMM in a larger sample size, as greater power may reveal even more significant differences in performance. Findings also emphasize the importance of viewing performance validity on a continuum rather than as a dichotomous pass/fail. Understanding the TOMM and how single errors may be indicative of poorer performance in a youth sample could help to reframe the way PVT results are interpreted in clinical and forensic settings.
Social participation (SP) represents a key goal in TBI rehabilitation, as it allows the individual to return to active and purposeful roles in the community. However, studies on predictors of SP specific to post-acute universally accessible specialized rehabilitation pathways following TBI are scarce. Our objectives were thus to 1) characterize a literature-based set of pre-injury, injury-related, and post-injury variables, as well as SP (measured with the MPAI-4 Participation scale) in individuals participating in inpatient-outpatient or outpatient rehabilitation pathways within a universally accessible and organized trauma continuum of care and 2) assess the use of these variables to predict SP outcome after TBI for each rehabilitation pathway.
Participants and Methods:
Participants (N = 372) were adults admitted between 2016 and 2020 to an inpatient-outpatient rehabilitation pathway or an outpatient rehabilitation pathway after sustaining a mild, moderate or severe TBI. The French-Canadian adaptation of the MPAI-4 questionnaire (Malec, 2005; Guerrette & McKerral, 2021; McKerral et al., 2014) presents three subscales (Abilities, Adjustment, Participation) and a total score, assessing functional and SP outcome after TBI. The MPAI-4 is completed for all individuals at admission and discharge in three major rehabilitation centers in the greater Montreal region. Independent variables consisted of sociodemographic and clinical characteristics collected from medical files and rehabilitation databases. Outcome measures consisted of a general SP level (MPAI-4 Participation subscale score at discharge from outpatient rehabilitation) and productivity status at discharge (productive if employed, child rearing, homemaker, student or volunteering; unproductive if unemployed or retired). Multiple and logistic regressions investigated the predictive value of each variable for SP outcome and productivity, separately for each rehabilitation pathway.
Results:
Samples’ sociodemographic and clinical characteristics differed between rehabilitation pathways. The inpatient-outpatient sample presented older age, lower productivity before TBI and poorer SP levels at admission and discharge from rehabilitation. However, both samples showed significant improvement on SP levels during rehabilitation. For the inpatient-outpatient rehabilitation path, general SP outcome was significantly predicted by three variables (education years, MPAI-4 Ability and Adjustment scores at rehabilitation intake; R2 = 49%), whereas productivity status at discharge was significantly predicted by age at time of injury (R2 = 72%). For the outpatient rehabilitation path, general SP outcome was significantly predicted by five variables: premorbid hypertension, mental health diagnosis, total indirect rehabilitation hours received, MPAI-4 Abilities and Adjustment scores at rehabilitation intake (R2 = 47%), while productivity status at discharge was significantly predicted by age at time of injury and education years (R2 = 44%).
Conclusions:
Different TBI rehabilitation care pathways showed distinct sample characteristics, as well as different premorbid and post-injury predictors of SP outcome and productivity. This highlights the importance of being aware of the potential limited generalizability of previously identified predictors when extrapolated to different clinical, rehabilitation and sociocultural contexts. The predictive models obtained could help clinicians identify more accurately patients at risk of showing poorer SP and productivity outcomes at end of rehabilitation, influence intervention approaches put forward with these individuals and set more appropriate goals and expectations.
The increasing complexity of social stress may be especially threatening to mental health during childhood and adolescence. One's skills in effectively coping with this stress may contribute to symptoms of pediatric anxiety and depression, a growing, significant, and pervasive public health concern. In addition to strategic skills, individual differences in coping may reflect differences in brain structure, including the white matter pathways that integrate frontal lobe networks with those involved in social functioning. Identifying the neurological substrates underlying anxiety and depression is an important way to delineate mechanisms underlying development of these disorders. Deterministic automated-fiber quantification (AFQ) is a technique that removes potential error from manual tracking of white matter, segregating tracts into distinct nodes—diminishing the effect of crossing fibers—and quantifying the number of fibers in a tract, allowing for assessment of connectivity across regions. Collectively, this investigation aims to quantify the interplay between anxiety, depression, coping with social stress, and white matter microstructure in children and adolescents.
Participants and Methods:
Ninety-two healthy children and adolescents (8-17 years old; n=53 female, Mage=12.96; n=39 male, Mage=12.31) and a parent rated symptoms of anxiety and depression using the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC-III). Coping and stress reactivity were assessed using the Responses to Stress Questionnaire, Social Stress version. Children and adolescents also completed 64-direction DTI in a Siemens 3T Prisma scanner. White matter microstructure was quantified using AFQ; Fractional anisotropy (FA) values were extracted for 18 tracts, comprised of 100 nodes each.
Results:
Mean levels of parent- and self-reported anxiety and depression fell within the normative range, and children reported mild- to moderate social stress. Higher levels of social stress were associated with increased parent reported anxiety (r=.294, p=.002) and parent- and self-reported depression (r=.481, p<.001; r=.211, p=.034, respectively). Anxiety and depression were not significantly related to white matter microstructure; however, several specific links with coping were noted. Use of secondary control coping (e.g., cognitive restructuring) was associated with higher FA of the bilateral inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi (left IFOF r=.228, p=.027; right IFOF r=.299, p=.003) and left inferior longitudinal fasciculus (r=.269, p=.009); use of primary control coping (e.g., problem solving) was associated with higher FA of the bilateral uncinate fasciculi (left UF r=.216, p=.036; right UF r=.207, p=.045). Furthermore, use of primary and secondary control coping were associated with fewer symptoms of anxiety and depression, whereas greater use of disengagement coping (e.g., wishful thinking) was associated with more depressive symptoms.
Conclusions:
These findings highlight links among white matter microstructure in tracts integrating frontal with temporal and occipital regions, and adoption of adaptive (i.e., primary and secondary control) coping responses. This may suggest that strong connections between brain regions supports more of a modulatory than a neglecting coping strategy. Finding also replicate extant literature on the ties between coping style and psychosocial distress. Given that coping responses are amenable to intervention, capitalizing on these brain-behavior links during ongoing neuromaturation is worthy of future research, with a goal of reducing symptoms of anxiety and depression via the brain's support of adaptive coping.
Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) are neurodevelopmental disorders with overlapping symptomatology and shared genetic makeup. Numerous previous studies have investigated ASD and ADHD using resting state functional networks. One functional network of particular interest is the Default Mode Network (DMN), as it has been shown to be abnormal in several mental disorders. Previous studies have investigated the DMN in ASD and ADHD separately but reported mixed trends of increased and decreased functional connectivity (FC) in the DMN in ASD and increased FC in ADHD. Additionally, little studies have investigated executive and attentional network dysfunction in the DMN for ASD and ADHD populations. To better understand the shared characteristics between ASD and ADHD, this study analyzed the DMN FC in children with ASD and ADHD.
Participants and Methods:
Archival datasets from Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE)-I and ADHD-200 datasets were used, with 33 ADHD, 35 ASD, and 32 typically developing (TD) males (ages = 7-17 years). After applying a standard pre-processing pipeline, 11 regions of interest (ROIs) from the Dosenbach-160 atlas were examined with 55 ROI pairs generated for the 100 subjects.
Results:
Significant differences were noted between ASD-ADHD groups in attentional networks and executive functioning networks. Specifically, significant Group x VIQ interactions were noted for FC between the following pairs of regions: medial prefrontal cortex - ventromedial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex -ventromedial prefrontal cortex, inferior temporal lobe - ventromedial prefrontal cortex, angular -ventromedial prefrontal cortex, angular -anterior cingulate cortex, inferior temporal lobe -ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, angular -superior frontal lobe, and intraparietal sulcus -superior frontal lobe. In the above FC pairs, FC in ADHD was negatively correlated with VIQ, with no correlation for ASD and positive correlation for TD. Previous literature has indicated that ADHD individuals demonstrate increased executive functioning deficits compared to ASD individuals. This study provides evidence at a neural level for these findings by demonstrating decreased FC trends in ADHD in attentional and executive functioning networks compared to ASD individuals. Group and VIQ main effects demonstrated mixed patterns across the three groups, as well as shared decreased FC in attention/executive networks for both ASD and ADHD groups.
Conclusions:
In summary, this study found similar findings from previous studies regarding mixed connectivity patterns, as well as shared dysfunction between ASD and ADHD groups. These results help in solidifying the theory that ASD and ADHD share clinical and neural patterns which need to be examined further. Future directions include utilizing more ASD+ADHD comorbid individuals in studies comparing ASD and ADHD FC trends as well as seeking to further understand the neuropsychological and neuroimaging profiles in ASD and ADHD.
Executive functioning (EF) is impaired in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and EF challenges are exacerbated in youth with ASD and ADHD (ASD+ADHD), which may impact diagnostic timing. We hypothesized youth with ASD+ADHD would be more impaired in EF (as opposed to other functional domains) compared to autistic youth without ADHD (ASD-only), with particular deficits in metacognition and inhibition. We also predicted youth with ASD+ADHD would be at significant risk for delayed ASD diagnosis and that greater EF challenges would predict earlier age of ASD diagnosis across groups.
Participants and Methods:
Data from a clinical database was extracted for 400 youth who received a new diagnosis of ASD after age 5, either with a co-occurring diagnosis of ADHD (ASD+ADHD group: n=297; Mage of ASD diagnosis=10.49; 25.9% female; 48.1% white) or without a co-occurring ADHD diagnosis (ASD-only group: n=100; Mage of ASD diagnosis=12.02; 34.0% female; 44.7% white). EF was measured with the BRIEF-2 parent-report, and ASD symptom strength was measured with the SRS-2 School Age form. Independent samples t-tests investigated whether a) the ASD+ADHD group was uniquely impaired in EF compared to the ASD-only group, b) parents of ASD+ADHD report elevated EF problems, and c) the ASD+ADHD group was at significant risk for delayed ASD diagnosis. Pearson correlations examined the association between age of ASD diagnosis and EF for each diagnostic group. Hierarchical linear regressions further analyzed whether specific EF domains concurrently predicted age of ASD diagnosis, after controlling for the known predictors of assigned sex at birth, FSIQ, and ASD symptom strength.
Results:
The ASD+ADHD group had greater challenges in overall EF (t=-6.42, p<.001), metacognitive skills (t=-6.47, p <.001), and inhibition skills (t=-7.06, p<.001). There was no significant difference in parent-reported autism symptoms between the ASD and ASD+ADHD groups (t=0.973, p=.331). The ASD+ADHD group received ASD diagnoses earlier than the ASD-only group (t=4.194, p<.001). In the ASD-only group, age of ASD diagnosis was not significantly correlated to overall EF, metacognitive skills, nor inhibition skills (ps>.05). In the ASD+ADHD group, ASD diagnosis was significantly related to overall EF (r(297)=.128, p=.027) and metacognitive skills (r(297)=.329, p<.001) but not inhibition skills (r(297)=.078, p=.180). Hierarchical linear regressions controlling for assigned sex at birth, FSIQ, and SRS-2 T-scores were used to determine whether these EF components significantly predicted age of ASD diagnosis. Overall EF did not predict age of ASD diagnosis in the ASD+ADHD group (ß=.034, t=1.417, p=.157), but metacognitive skills did (ß=.123, t=5.582, p<.001).
Conclusions:
Our findings suggest youth with ASD+ADHD have greater impairment in overall EF, metacognition, and inhibition compared to ASD-only youth, despite similar levels of ASD traits, consistent with hypotheses. Contrary to our hypothesis, youth with ASD+ADHD in this sample were diagnosed with ASD earlier. However, results also suggest EF problems, specifically metacognitive deficits, predict later age of ASD diagnosis. Future research is needed to replicate findings and better understand how EF and other functional domains predict ASD diagnostic timing.
Social functioning patterns vary across measures in children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1; Glad et al., 2021) with broad psychosocial screening measures having shown no impairment (Klein-Tasman et al., 2014; Martin et al., 2012; Sangster et al., 2011) while a more specific social functioning measure indicated poorer social skills (Barton & North, 2004; Huijbregts & de Sonneville, 2011; Loitfelder et al., 2015). The current aims were to characterize caregiver-reported social skills using three different measures and determine which measure appears to best capture social difficulties for young children with NF1.
Participants and Methods:
Fifty children with NF1 (31 males; M=3.96, SD=1.05) and 20 unaffected siblings (11 males; M=4.34, SD=0.88) in early childhood (ages 3-6) were rated by a caregiver on one social functioning measure (the Social Skills scale on the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS)) and two broader functioning measures that include assessment of social functioning (the Social Skills scale on the Behavior Assessment System for Children-Second Edition (BASC-2), Social Interaction and Communication domain on the Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised (SIB-R)).
Results:
For children with NF1, the SSRS mean standard score was significantly lower than the BASC-2 and SIB-R (f=-5.11, p<.001; f=-4.63, p<.001) while there was no significant difference between the BASC-2 and SIB-R. No significant differences emerged between measures for unaffected siblings. No significant group differences in mean standard score were found for the SSRS, BASC-2 or SIB-R. Fisher’s exact tests revealed the NF1 group had significantly more frequent difficulties than unaffected siblings on the BASC-2 (p=.017) but not on the SSRS or SIB-R. For both groups, Cochran’s Q tests determined a significant difference in the proportion of identified social difficulties across measures (NF1: X2(2)=16.33, p<.001; Siblings: X2(2)=9.25, p=.01). Follow up McNemar’s tests demonstrated significantly more difficulties reported on the SSRS compared to the BASC-2 for both groups (NF1: p<.001; Siblings: p=.016). Significantly more frequent difficulties were also reported on the SSRS compared to the SIB-R for the NF1 group (p=.002) but not for the unaffected siblings group. No difference in the frequency of difficulties was evident between the BASC-2 and SIB-R for either group.
Conclusions:
Social skills difficulties appear to be best captured using the SSRS in young children, particularly for children with NF1 as this measure resulted in the lowest mean score and the greatest frequency of difficulties observed within the NF1 group. However, it is notable that group differences in comparison to unaffected siblings were not observed in mean score or frequency of difficulties, such that these young children with NF1 are not showing marked social challenges but rather, social difficulties may be mild when present at this age. Nevertheless, using a measure that specifically targets social functioning, rather than a measure where social functioning is merely a component of a broad measure, appears beneficial to capturing social difficulty. Using measures that best capture social difficulties will contribute to early identification and assessment of intervention effectiveness. Further work with additional age ranges and longitudinal trajectory is needed.
Larger social networks are linked to better cognitive function. However, little is known about the association of cognitive function with the composition of these networks in terms of the varying levels of closeness and supportive relationships. The present study explored whether social network size at different levels was differentially associated with cognitive function in a group of community-dwelling older adults.
Participants and Methods:
119 older adults (Mage= 70.71) from the Maine Aging Behavior Learning Enrichment Study completed a neuropsychological test battery measuring language, verbal memory, visuospatial memory, working memory, executive function, and processing speed abilities. The number and closeness of participants' relationships was measured using a Hierarchical Mapping Technique based on the Social Convoy model, in which participants included the names of people in their relational network within inner (closest), middle (close), and outer concentric circles.
Results:
Correlational analyses found that social network size at the total and middle-to-outer levels were statistically associated with education and better performance on measures of language, verbal memory, visuospatial memory, and executive function. However, no relationship emerged between the size of the innermost network level and cognitive function. Furthermore, statistically significant findings did not survive adjustments for the effect of education.
Conclusions:
Broader levels of support, rather than greater intimacy, were statistically associated with better cognitive performance. Consistent with previous research, greater education was associated with larger social networks. Future research is needed to understand whether higher levels of education or other factors mediate the observed relationship.
Assessment of response validity is essential to neuropsychological assessment. Although informant report of examinee functioning has previously been associated with examinee-generated performance and cognitive symptom invalidity (PVT; SVT-C), empirically-derived guidelines for interpreting informantreport validity are lacking. This study sought to assess the classification accuracy of a widely used informant-report measure, the Dementia Severity Rating Scale (DSRS), for discriminating examinee-generated PVT and SVT-C.
Participants and Methods:
Data were collected from 145 examinee-informant dyads who completed neuropsychological batteries as part of a routine workup in an epilepsy monitoring unit. PVT status was determined by below-threshold performances on >2 indicators (Test of Memory Malingering, Wechsler Digit Span Age Corrected Scaled Score, Word Memory Test). SVT-C status was determined by above-threshold responses on both the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2-Restructured Form Response Bias Scale (MMPI-2-RF RBS) and Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology Amnestic Disorders subscale (SIMS-AM). After assessing demographic and relational covariance via t-test and chi square analyses, receiver operator characteristic curves were derived to assess the classification accuracy of the DSRS for discriminating examinee PVT and SVT-C status.
Results:
DSRS total score demonstrated acceptable accuracy in classifying PVT status (AUC = .77), with cut scores of >21 and >15 yielding .93-.82 specificity and .44-.63 sensitivity. The DSRS also classified SVT-C status with acceptable accuracy (AUC = .71), with the aforementioned cut scores exhibiting .90-.78 specificity and .50-.64 sensitivity. The DSRS also classified SVT-C status using only one indicator (i.e., MMPI-2-RF RBS or SIMS-AM) with acceptable accuracy (AUC = .71-.72), with the aforementioned cut scores exhibiting .92 specificity and .37-.42 sensitivity.
Conclusions:
The DSRS can be used to classify examinee-generated PVT and SVT-C on an epilepsy monitoring unit. Results provide empirically-derived psychometric guidelines for interpreting informant-report response validity that are clinically useful and lay the groundwork for future investigations of informant-report response validity.
Central nervous system (CNS) infections with the dematiaceous fungus Cladophialophora bantiana (C. bantiana) are extremely rare, with approximately 120 confirmed cases reported as of 2016. C. bantiana is a highly neurotropic and thermotolerant fungus found in soil worldwide. The mode of entry into the CNS remains unknown, but inhalation of fungal spores or subcutaneous trauma have been suggested. Entry of fungal spores can cause cerebral phaeohyphomycosis with the main clinical manifestation of a brain abscess. Symptoms are non-specific and can include headache, fever, hemiparesis, aphasia, visual disturbances, and confusion. C. bantiana cerebral phaeohyphomycosis occurs in both immunocompetent and immunocompromised individuals, with a slightly higher prevalence in immunocompetent males for unknown reasons. Diagnosis is often delayed due to its nonspecific presentation and prevalence in individuals without pre-existing immunological disease. Prognosis is poor, with mortality rates of approximately 70% despite aggressive treatment. Treatment is not standardized but may include several anti-fungal agents and surgical intervention. Case reports documenting the variability seen with cerebral phaeohyphomycosis by C. bantiana can provide valuable insight into this emerging disease. C. bantiana’s neurotropic propensity also warrants cognitive investigation of the disease; however, there are currently limited descriptions of cognitive findings in published case reports of C. bantiana CNS infections.
Participants and Methods:
Here, we describe a case of a 35-year-old immunocompetent, college educated male with a CNS C. bantiana infection, presumably following a fall while biking in Costa Rica. First symptoms included left sided facial palsy, headache, and hand weakness, prompting extensive diagnostic workup, with diagnosis of C. bantiana infection confirmed 8 months after symptom onset. Initial treatment included anti-fungal agents and steroids, but his course of infection was complicated by infectious vasculitis with posterior circulation infarcts and obstructive hydrocephalus requiring ventriculoperitoneal shunt placement two years following the fungal infection diagnosis. The most recent brain MRI revealed encephalomalacia in global periventricular areas, two small masses, likely representing small fungal phlegmons, and enhancing lesions in the upper cervical spinal canal.
Results:
The patient reported cognitive changes following the infarcts and shunt placement including difficulties with spatial navigation, following directions, and articulating thoughts. Memory concerns and lapses in judgment were also reported. Results from a neuropsychological evaluation revealed high average baseline intellectual abilities with decrements in visuospatial processing, processing speed, executive functioning, and aspects of memory stemming from his executive dysfunction. At the time, his cognitive profile suggested parietal and frontosubcortical systems disruption meeting criteria for mild cognitive impairment. Two years later, the patient reported continuing cognitive difficulties prompting a follow-up neuropsychological evaluation. Results were similar to his first evaluation, revealing deficits in aspects of visuospatial processing, decreased verbal and visual learning, bradyphrenia and processing speed deficits, and difficulties with visual planning and organization. Minimal anxiety and depression, but increased apathy and executive dysfunction were endorsed on self-report measures.
Conclusions:
This case report highlights neurological sequela resulting from CNS infection with C. bantiana, -with a course complicated by subsequent strokes, hydrocephalus, and cognitive impairment-, and contributes additional insight into the relatively limited existing reports of an extremely rare but emerging disease.
Epilepsy includes recurrent, unprovoked seizures and affects 470,000 children in the US, of which 7% have drug-resistant epilepsy due to failing two or more antiseizure medication trials. For some patients with drug-resistant epilepsy, surgery has been successful in reducing seizure burden. Functional MRI (fMRI) and intracranial mapping of neurocognitive functions, especially language, are increasingly done to assess potential functional loss from epilepsy surgery. However, these procedures vary by medical institute. The purpose of this review was to examine published literature on fMRI and intracranial mapping procedures for pediatric epilepsy surgery workup toward development of a standardized protocol that can be used across institutes as a guide to standard-of-care best practices for predicting loss of function associated with epilepsy surgery.
Participants and Methods:
Our literature review includes information from 8 electronic databases for peer-reviewed, English language studies of evaluation for pediatric epilepsy surgery candidacy. Thirty-one studies were selected based on inclusion criteria. Only studies including fMRI and intracranial mapping conducted with pediatric patients being worked up for epilepsy surgery were selected.
Results:
Our review revealed that the most common task used in fMRI and intracranial mapping procedures is visual-object naming, but type of naming tasks and the way they are administered varies widely across medical institutes and includes published measures and those created on site. Variability makes examining findings across studies and designing best practice for these procedures challenging.
Conclusions:
Creating gold-standard procedures for fMRI and intracranial mapping administration for epilepsy surgery evaluations is critical in optimizing treatment and functional outcomes for our pediatric patients. Our review is an initial step in this process.
Perceived cognitive dysfunction is a common feature of late-life depression (LLD) that is associated with diminished quality of life and greater disability. Similar associations have been demonstrated in individuals with Hoarding Disorder. The degree to which hoarding behaviors (HB) are associated with greater perceived cognitive dysfunction and disability in individuals with concurrent LLD is not known.
Participants and Methods:
Participants with LLD (N=83) completed measures of hoarding symptom severity (Savings Inventory-Revised; SI-R) and were classified into two groups based on HB severity: LLD+HB who exhibited significant HB (SI-R . 41, n = 25) and LLD with low HB (SI-R < 41, n = 58). Additional measures assessed depression severity (Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HDRS), perceived cognitive difficulties (Everyday Cognition Scale; ECOG), and disability (World Health Organization Disability Assessment Scale [WHODAS]-II-Short). Given a non-normal distribution of ECOG and WHODAS-II scores, non-parametric Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney tests were used to assess group differences in perceived cognitive dysfunction and disability. A regression model assessed the extent to which perceived cognitive dysfunction was associated with hoarding symptom severity measured continuously, covarying for age, education, gender, and depression severity. A separate regression model assessed the extent to which disability scores were associated with perceived cognitive dysfunction and HB severity covarying for demographics and depression severity.
Results:
LLD+HB endorsed significantly greater perceived cognitive dysfunction (W = 1023, p = 0.003) and greater disability (W = 1006, p = < 0.001) compared to LLD. Regression models accounting for demographic characteristics and depression severity revealed that greater HB severity was associated with greater perceived cognitive dysfunction (β = 0.009, t = 2.765, p = 0.007). Increased disability was associated with greater perceived cognitive dysfunction (β = 4.792, t(71) = 3.551, p = 0.0007) and HB severity (β = 0.080, t(71) = 1.944, p = 0.056) approached significance after accounting for variance explained by depression severity and demographic covariates.
Conclusions:
Our results suggest that hoarding behaviors are associated with increased perceived cognitive dysfunction and greater disability in individuals with LLD. Screening for HB in individuals with LLD may help identify those at greater risk for poor cognitive and functional outcomes. Interventions that target HB and perceived cognitive difficulties may decrease risk for disability in LLD. However, longitudinal studies would be required to further evaluate these relationships.
Essential tremor (ET) is the most common movement disorder, characterized by bilateral action tremors of the upper extremities. Surgical interventions can be considered for severe cases that are refractory to medication. Magnetic resonance-guided focused ultrasound (MRgFUS) of the ventral intermediate nucleus of the thalamus (Vim) is a recently approved, minimally invasive treatment for unilateral tremor. While patients are generally pleased with unilateral treatment, many patients are bothered by tremor on the untreated side. Historically, bilateral thalamotomy has been associated with a higher rate of adverse events, including cognitive impairment. MRgFUS Vim thalamotomy for bilateral tremor is currently being investigated. The goal of the present study was to evaluate the effect of bilateral MRgFUS Vim thalamotomy on cognition.
Participants and Methods:
Twelve patients with medication-refractory essential tremor (mean age = 68.77 +/- 11.78 years, mean education = 14.34 +/- 2.71 years, 8 male) were included in the present study. Three of the 12 patients met criteria for mild cognitive impairment (MCI). All patients successfully underwent unilateral MRgFUS thalamotomy at least 48 weeks before the second thalamotomy. A battery of neuropsychological tests was administered to patients before (considered baseline in the present study) and three months following the second thalamotomy. Baseline evaluations occurred on average 144.64 +/- 91.53 weeks (range: 55.00 - 346.58) after the first thalamotomy. The neuropsychological battery assessed domains of processing speed (Oral Symbol Digit Modalities Test, D-KEFS Color-Word Naming and Reading), attention (WAIS-IV Digit Span Forward), executive function (D-KEFS Color-Word Inhibition and Inhibition/Switching), working memory (WAIS-IV Digit Span Backward and Sequencing), verbal fluency (D-KEFS Letter Fluency and Animal Fluency), confrontation naming (Boston Naming Test), verbal memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised), and visuospatial perception (Judgment of Line Orientation). Alternate versions of tests were used when possible. Cognitive changes were analyzed at the group and individual level. Group level changes were assessed with paired sample t-tests (corrected for multiple comparisons). At the individual level, postoperative declines > 1.5 SD from baseline were considered clinically significant.
Results:
Participants’ baseline intellectual functioning ranged from low average to superior (as measured by the WTAR). The mean baseline score on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment was 24.58 (range: 17 - 30). At the group level, there were no significant changes in cognitive scores from baseline to follow-up (all p values > 0.635). At the individual level, one patient with MCI declined > 1.5 SD on the verbal memory composite. No other patients showed declines > 1.5 SD.
Conclusions:
Our preliminary findings suggest that bilateral MRgFUS Vim thalamotomy is relatively safe from a cognitive perspective. However, a single patient with MCI exhibited clinically significant postoperative decline in verbal memory. Future studies with larger sample sizes are needed to investigate the factors that increase the risk of postoperative cognitive decline, including pre-existing cognitive impairment, older age, and lesion size.
Childhood ischemic and hemorrhagic stroke is often associated with neuropsychological and cognitive deficits. Stroke induces an inflammatory response in the central and peripheral nervous systems. High levels of inflammatory markers in the plasma have been associated with poorer cognitive outcomes. The role of inflammation in neurological prognosis of stroke has been studied previously; however, there is a limited understanding of the association between inflammatory markers and neuropsychological outcome post-stroke. The present review examined the existing literature on the association between inflammatory markers and post-stroke functioning.
Participants and Methods:
Data bases (PsycINFO, PubMed, Web of Science, and Ovid) were reviewed in October 2020. Articles were restricted to English-language literature. Articles were included regardless of recruitment setting, number of strokes, mechanism of stroke, timing of blood collection and outcome assessment. The articles focused on patients with stroke (between the ages of 0 to 95), measured post-stroke outcome by neurological and cognitive outcome measures (i.e., it included findings on any aspect of cognition such as memory, information processing, or attention), and on pro-inflammatory cytokines and c-reactive proteins as measures of inflammation. The systematic literature search retrieved 954 articles to review against inclusion criteria. Descriptive statistics were performed using IBM SPSS 27.0 Statistics Software.
Results:
A total of 18 articles were included in this review. The population age ranged from 21 to 95, and, when reported (n=17), mean participant age was 66.31. Among stroke patient populations, ischemic stroke was most researched (n=15). The most widely investigated biomarkers were CRP (n=9), IL-6 (n=8), TNF- a (n=7), IL-1 b (n=5), and IL-10 (n=5). The time of initial blood collection ranged from on admission to within 3 months poststroke. Equal number of studies used both neurological and cognitive tests (n=7), or only neurological (n=7), 2 studies only used cognitive tests, and one study used all three types of measures. The most commonly used cognitive test was the Mini Mental State Examination, MMSE (n=7). The next commonly used cognitive test was the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), (n=4). Only two studies used a comprehensive neuropsychological battery.
Conclusions:
There is a lack of research into diverse stroke populations. All the studies examined the association between inflammatory markers and the post-stroke outcomes in adult populations and mostly in patients with ischemic stroke. The lack of research on pediatric and young adult stroke represents a significant gap in understanding predictors of neurological and cognitive outcomes. Further, the review revealed a lack of comprehensive neurocognitive assessment post stroke, with most studies measuring neuropsychological outcome using brief cognitive instruments. Our findings highlight a critical need for addressing the above gaps to help elucidate the role of inflammatory markers in the neuropsychological prognosis of stroke in younger populations.