Skip to main content
×
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 1
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Gravett, Nadine Bhagwandin, Adhil Lyamin, Oleg I. Siegel, Jerome M. and Manger, Paul R. 2017. Sociality Affects REM Sleep Episode Duration Under Controlled Laboratory Conditions in the Rock Hyrax, Procavia capensis. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, Vol. 11, Issue. ,

    ×
  • Print publication year: 2009
  • Online publication date: March 2010

1 - Ecological constraints on mammalian sleep architecture

Summary

Introduction: sleep and ecology

All mammals so far studied experience some form of sleep. When mammals are sleep-deprived, they generally attempt to regain the lost sleep by exhibiting a “sleep rebound,” suggesting that sleep serves important functions that cannot be neglected (Siegel, 2008; Zepelin, 1989; Zepelin, Siegel, & Tobler, 2005). When sleep deprivation is enforced on individuals, it is accompanied by impaired physiological functions and a deterioration of cognitive performance (Kushida, 2004; Rechtschaffen, 1998; Rechtschaffen & Bergmann, 2002). In the rat, prolonged sleep deprivation ultimately results in death (Kushida, 2004; Rechtschaffen & Bergmann, 2002). Together, these observations suggest that sleep is a fundamental requirement for mammalian life, and much research has focused on identifying the physiological benefits that sleep provides (Horne, 1988; Kushida, 2004).

Are there also costs associated with sleep? If so, what are the selective pressures that constrain the amount of time that individuals can devote to sleep? Sleep is probably associated with “opportunity costs” because sleeping animals cannot pursue other fitness-enhancing activities, such as locating food, maintaining social bonds, or finding mates. Sleeping animals may also pay direct costs. For example, sleep is a state of reduced consciousness, and thus sleeping individuals are less able to detect and escape from approaching predators (Allison & Cicchetti, 1976; Lima, Rattenborg, Lesku, et al., 2005). These ecological factors are likely to be important constraints on sleep durations and may also affect how sleep is organized over the daily cycle.

Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

Evolution of Sleep
  • Online ISBN: 9780511642074
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511642074
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×
References
Acerbi, A., McNamara, P., & Nunn, C. L. (2008). To sleep or not to sleep: The ecology of sleep in artificial organisms. BMC Ecology, 8, 10.
Affani, J. M., Cervino, C. O., & Marcos, H. J. A. (2001). Absence of penile erections during paradoxical sleep. Peculiar penile events during wakefulness and slow-wave sleep in the armadillo. Journal of Sleep Research, 10, 219–228.
Allison, T., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1976). Sleep in mammals: Ecological and constitutional correlates. Science, 194, 732–734.
Ambrosini, M. V., & Giuditta, A. (2001). Learning and sleep: The sequential hypothesis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5, 477–490.
Ball, N. J. (1992). The phasing of sleep in animals. In Stampi, C. (Ed.), Why we nap. Evolution, chronobiology, and functions of polyphasic and ultrashort sleep (pp. 31–49). Boston: Birkhäser.
Barre, V., & Petter-Rousseaux, A. (1988). Seasonal variation in sleep-wake cycle in Microcebus murinus. Primates, 29, 53–64.
Benington, J. H., & Heller, H. C. (1994). Does the function of REM sleep concern non-REM sleep or waking?Progress in Neurobiology, 44, 433–449.
Benington, J. H., & Heller, H. C. (1995). Restoration of brain energy metabolism as the function of sleep. Progress in Neurobiology, 45, 347–360.
Berger, R. J. (1990). Relations between sleep duration, body weight and metabolic rate in mammals. Animal Behaviour, 40, 989–991.
Berger, R. J., & Walker, J. M. (1972). A polygraphic study of sleep in the tree shrew (Tupaia glis). Brain Behavior and Evolution, 5, 54–69.
Bert, J., Balzamo, E., Chase, M., & Pegram, V. (1975). Sleep of baboon, Papio papio, under natural conditions and in laboratory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 39, 657–662.
Bert, J., Pegram, V., Rhodes, J. M., Balzamo, E., & Naquet, R. (1970). A comparative study of two Cercopithecinae. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 28, 32–40.
Blackburn, T. M., & Hawkins, B. A. (2004). Bergmann's rule and the mammal fauna of northern North America. Ecography, 27, 715–724.
Blomberg, S. P., & Garland, T. (2002). Tempo and mode in evolution: Phylogenetic inertia, adaptation and comparative methods. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 15, 899–910.
Blomberg, S. P., Garland, T., & Ives, A. R. (2003). Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: Behavioural traits are more labile. Evolution, 57, 717–745.
Broughton, R. J. (1973). Confusional sleep disorders: Interrelationship with memory consolidation and retrieval in sleep. In Boag, T. & Campbell, D. (Eds.), A triune concept of the brain and behaviour (pp. 115–127). Toronto: Toronto University Press.
Bryant, P. A., Trinder, J., & Curtis, N. (2004). Sick and tired: Does sleep have a vital role in the immune system?Nature Reviews, Immunology, 4, 457–467.
Campbell, S. S., & Tobler, I. (1984). Animal sleep: A review of sleep duration across phylogeny. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 8, 269–300.
Capellini, I., Barton, R. A., McNamara, P., Preston, B. T., & Nunn, C. L. (2008a). Phylogenetic analysis of ecology and evolution of mammalian sleep. Evolution, 62, 1764–1776.
Capellini, I., Nunn, C. L., McNamara, P., Preston, B. T., & Barton, R. A. (2008b). Energetic constraints, not predation, influence the evolution of sleep patterning in mammals. Functional Ecology, 22(5), 847–853.
Caro, T. (2005). Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Elgar, M. A., Pagel, M. D., & Harvey, P. H. (1988). Sleep in mammals. Animal Behaviour, 36, 1407–1419.
Felsenstein, J. (1985). Phylogenies and the comparative method. The American Naturalist, 125, 1–15.
Fenn, M. G. P., & Macdonald, D. W. (1995). Use of middens by red foxes: Risk reverses rhythms of rats. Journal of Mammalogy, 76, 130–136.
Freckleton, R. P., Harvey, P. H., & Pagel, M. (2002). Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence. The American Naturalist, 160, 712–726.
Fuchs, T., Haney, A., Jechura, T. J., Moore, F. R., & Bingman, V. P. (2006). Daytime naps in night-migrating birds: Behavioural adaptation to seasonal sleep deprivation in the Swainson's thrush, Catharus ustulatus. Animal Behaviour, 72, 951–958.
Moura Filho, Galvão, G., A, Huggins, S. E., & Lines, S. G. (1983). Sleep and waking in the three-toed sloth, Bradypus tridactylus. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, A: Comparative Physiology, 76, 345–355.
Garland, T., Bennett, A. F., & Rezende, E. L. (2005). Phylogenetic approaches in comparative physiology. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 208, 3015–3035.
Gauthier-Clerc, M., Tamisier, A., & Cezilly, F. (1998). Sleep-vigilance trade-off in green-winged teals (Anas crecca crecca). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 76, 2214–2218.
Gauthier-Clerc, M., Tamisier, A., & Cezilly, F. (2000). Sleep-vigilance trade-off in gadwall during the winter period. Condor, 102, 307–313.
Gauthier-Clerc, M., Tamisier, A., & Cezilly, F. (2002). Vigilance while sleeping in the breeding pochard Aythya ferina according to sex and age. Bird Study, 49, 300–303.
Harvey, P. A., & Pagel, M. (1991). The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Horne, J. A. (1988). Why we sleep: The functions of sleep in humans and other animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Jouvet-Monier, D., & Astic, L. (1966). Study of sleep in the adult and newborn guinea pig. Comptes Rendus des Séances de la Société de Biologie et de ses Filiales (Paris), 160, 1453–1457.
Kushida, C. A. (2004). Sleep deprivation: Basic science, physiology, and behavior. (Lung Biology in Health and Disease). New York: Marcel Dekker.
Lendrem, D. W. (1983). Sleeping and vigilance in birds. I. Field observations of the mallard (Anas platyrhynchos). Animal Behaviour, 31, 532–538.
Lendrem, D. W. (1984). Sleeping and vigilance in birds. II. An experimental study of the barbary dove (Streptopelia risoria). Animal Behaviour, 32, 243–248.
Lesku, J. A., Bark, R. J., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C. J., & Lima, S. L. (2008). Predator-induced plasticity in sleep architecture in wild-caught Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). Behavioural Brain Research, 189, 298–305.
Lesku, J. A., Roth, T. C., Amlaner, C. J., & Lima, S. L. (2006). A phylogenetic analysis of sleep architecture in mammals: The integration of anatomy, physiology, and ecology. The American Naturalist, 168, 441–453.
Lima, S. L., Rattenborg, N. C., Lesku, J. A., & Amlaner, C. J. (2005). Sleeping under the risk of predation. Animal Behaviour, 70, 723–736.
Lindstedt, S. L., & Boyce, M. S. (1984). Seasonality, fasting endurance, and body size in mammals. The American Naturalist, 125, 873–878.
Macdonald, D. (2006). The encyclopedia of mammals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Majde, J. A. (2005). Links between the innate immune system and sleep. The Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 116, 1188–1198.
Makeig, S., Jung, T. P., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Awareness during drowsiness: Dynamics and electrophysiological correlates. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 266–273.
Martins, E., & Garland, T. (1991). Phylogenetic analyses of the correlated evolution of continuous characters: A simulation study. Evolution, 45, 534–557.
McNamara, P., Capellini, I., Harris, E., Nunn, C. L., Barton, R. A., & Preston, B. T. (2008). The Phylogeny of Sleep Database: A new resource for sleep scientists. The Open Sleep Journal, 1, 11–14.
Meddis, R. (1975). On the function of sleep. Animal Behaviour, 23, 676–691.
Mukhametov, L. M. (1984). Sleep in marine mammals. Experimental Brain Research, 8, 227–238.
Mukhametov, L. M. (1995). Paradoxical sleep peculiarities in aquatic mammals. Sleep Research, 24A, 202.
Noser, R., Gygax, L., & Tobler, I. (2003). Sleep and social status in captive gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada). Behavioral Brain Research, 147, 9–15.
Nunn, C. L., & Barton, R. A. (2001). Comparative methods for studying primate adaptation and allometry. Evolutionary Anthropology, 10, 81–98.
Oates, J. F. (1987). Food distribution and foraging behavior. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 197–209). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Palchykova, S., Deboer, T., & Tobler, I. (2003). Seasonal aspects of sleep in the Djungarian hamster. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 9–17.
Pillay, P., & Manger, P. R. (2004). Testing thermogenesis as the basis for the evolution of sleep phenomenology. Journal of Sleep Research, 13, 353–358.
Preston, B. T., Capellini, I., McNamara, P., Barton, R. A., & Nunn, C. L. (2009). Parasite resistance and the adaptive significance of sleep. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 9, 7.
Rattenborg, N. C., & Amlaner, C. J. (2002). Phylogeny of sleep. In Lee-Chiong, T. L., Sateia, M. J., & Carskadon, M. A. (Eds.), Sleep medicine (pp. 7–22). Philadelphia: Hanley & Belfus, Inc.
Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C. J., & Lima, S. L. (2000). Behavioral, neurophysiological, and evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 817–842.
Rattenborg, N. C., Lima, E. M., & Amlaner, C. J. (1999a). Half-awake to the risk of predation. Nature, 397, 397–398.
Rattenborg, N. C., Lima, S. L., & Amlaner, C. J. (1999b). Facultative control of avian unihemispheric sleep under the risk of predation. Behavioural Brain Research, 105, 163–172.
Rattenborg, N. C., Mandt, B. H., Obermeyer, W. H., Winsauer, P. J., Huber, R., Wikelski, M., et al. (2004). Migratory sleeplessness in the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii). PLoS Biology, 2, 0924–0936.
Rattenborg, N. C., Voirin, B., Vyssotski, A. L., Kays, R. W., Spoelstra, K., Kuemmeth, F., et al. (2008). Sleeping outside the box: Electroencephalographic measures of sleep in sloths inhabiting a rainforest. Biology Letters, 4(4), 402–405.
Rechtschaffen, A. (1998). Current perspectives on the function of sleep. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 41, 359–391.
Rechtschaffen, A., & Bergmann, B. M. (2002). Sleep deprivation in the rat: An update of the 1989 paper. Sleep, 25, 18–24.
Ruckebush, Y. (1963). Etude EEG et comportamentale des alternantes veille-sommeil chez lane [EEG and behavioral study of alternating waking and sleeping in the donkey]. Comptes Rendus des Séances de la Société de Biologie et de ses Filiales, 157, 840–844.
Saarikko, J. (1992). Risk of predation and foraging activity in shrews. Annales Zoologici Fennici, 29, 291–299.
Saarikko, J., & Hanski, I. (1990). Timing of rest and sleep in foraging shrews. Animal Behaviour, 26, 861–869.
Siegel, J. M. (2004). Sleep phylogeny: Clues to the evolution and function of sleep. In Luppi, P. H. (Ed.), Sleep: Circuits and functions (pp. 163–176). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Siegel, J. M. (2005). Clues to the function of mammalian sleep. Nature, 437, 1264–1271.
Siegel, J. M. (2008). Do all animals sleep?Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 208–213.
Stampi, C. (1992). Evolution, chronobiology, and functions of polyphasic and ultrashort sleep: Main issues. In Stampi, C. (Ed.), Why we nap. Evolution, chronobiology, and functions of polyphasic and ultrashort sleep (pp. 1–20). Boston: Birkhäser.
Steiger, A. (2003). Sleep and endocrinology. Journal of Internal Medicine, 254, 13–22.
Tobler, I. (1989). Napping and polyphasic sleep in mammals. In Dinges, D. F. & Broughton, R. J. (Eds.), Sleep and alertness: Chronobiological, behavioral, and medical aspects of napping (pp. 9–30). New York: Raven Press.
Tobler, I. (1995). Is sleep fundamentally different between mammalian species?Behavioural Brain Research, 69, 35–41.
Tobler, I. (2005). Phylogeny and sleep regulation. In Kryger, M. H., Roth, T., & Dement, W. C. (Eds.), Principles and practices of sleep medicine (pp. 72–81). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Ursin, R. (1968). The two stages of slow-wave sleep in the cat and their relation to REM sleep. Brain Research, 11, 347–356.
Cauter, E., Plat, L., & Copinschi, G. (1998). Interrelations between sleep and the somatotropic axis. Sleep, 21, 553–566.
Twyver, H. (1969). Sleep patterns in five rodent species. Physiology & Behavior, 4, 901–905.
Twyver, H., & Garrett, W. (1972). Arousal threshold in the rat determined by “meaningful” stimuli. Behavioral Biology, 7, 205–215.
Voss, U. (2004). Functions of sleep architecture and the concept of protective fields. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15, 33–46.
Wauquier, A., Verheyen, J. L., Broeck, W. A. E., & Janssen, P. A. J. (1979). Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 46, 33–48.
Withers, P. C. (1992). Comparative animal physiology. Orlando, FL: W. B. Saunders College Publishing.
Zepelin, H. (1989). Mammalian sleep. In Kryger, M. H., Roth, T., & Dement, W. C. (Eds.), Principles and practices of sleep medicine (pp. 30–49). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Zepelin, H., Siegel, J. M., & Tobler, I. (2005). Mammalian sleep. In Kryger, M. H., Roth, T., & Dement, W. C. (Eds.), Principles and practices of sleep medicine (pp. 91–100). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.