Skip to main content
×
×
Home

Passive frame theory: A new synthesis

  • Ezequiel Morsella (a1) (a2), Christine A. Godwin (a3), Tiffany K. Jantz (a4), Stephen C. Krieger (a5) and Adam Gazzaley (a2) (a6)...
Abstract

Passive frame theory attempts to illuminate what consciousness is, in mechanistic and functional terms; it does not address the “implementation” level of analysis (how neurons instantiate conscious states), an enigma for various disciplines. However, in response to the commentaries, we discuss how our framework provides clues regarding this enigma. In the framework, consciousness is passive albeit essential. Without consciousness, there would not be adaptive skeletomotor action.

Copyright
References
Hide All
Ach, N. (1905/1951) Determining tendencies: Awareness. In: Organization and pathology of thought, ed. Rapaport, D., pp. 1538. Columbia University Press (Original work published in 1905).
Adrian, E. D. (1942) Olfactory reactions in the brain of the hedgehog. The Journal of Physiology 100(4):459–73.
Adrian, E. D. (1950a) Sensory discrimination: With some recent evidence from the olfactory organ. British Medical Bulletin 6(4):330–32.
Adrian, E. D. (1950b) The electrical activity of the mammalian olfactory bulb. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 2(1):377–88.
Alkire, M., Hudetz, A. & Tononi, G. (2008) Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322(5903):876–80.
Allen, A. K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A. & Morsella, E. (2013) Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: A new experimental paradigm for consciousness research. Consciousness and Cognition 22:1318–31.
Allman, J. M. (2000) Evolving brains. Scientific American Library.
Allport, D. A. (1989) Visual attention. In: Foundations of cognitive science, vol. 2, ed. Posner, M. I., pp. 631–82. MIT Press.
Anderson, J. (1983) The architecture of cognition. Harvard University Press.
Aru, J. & Bachmann, T. (2009) Occipital EEG correlates of conscious awareness when subjective target shine-through and effective visual masking are compared: Bifocal early increase in gamma power and speed-up of P1. Brain Research 1271:6073.
Augustinova, M. & Ferrand, L. (2014) Automaticity of word reading: Evidence from the semantic Stroop paradigm. Current Directions in Psychological Science 23:343–48.
Baars, B. (1988) A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press.
Baars, B. J. (1997b) Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception, and working memory. Consciousness and Cognition 6:363–71.
Baars, B. J. (2002) The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(1):4752.
Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R. & Fiore, M. C. (2004) Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review 111:3351.
Banks, W. P. (1995) Evidence for consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 4:270–72.
Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmid, A. M., Dale, A. M., Hämäläinen, M. S., Marinkovic, K., Schacter, D. L., Rosen, B. R. & Halgren, E. (2006) Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103:449–54.
Bargh, J. A. & Morsella, E. (2008) The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3:7379.
Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (2004) Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. Guilford Press.
Beshel, J., Kopell, N. & Kay, L. M. (2007) Olfactory bulb gamma oscillations are enhanced with task demands. Journal of Neuroscience 27:8358–65.
Bhangal, S., Cho, H., Geisler, M. W. & Morsella, E. (2016) The prospective nature of voluntary action: Insights from the reflexive imagery task. Review of General Psychology 20:101–17.
Bindra, D. (1974) A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review 81:199213.
Bindra, D. (1978) How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:4191.
Block, N. (1995b) On a confusion about a function of consciousness. (Target article) Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18(2):227–47; discussion 247–87.
Block, N. (2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30:481548.
Boveroux, P., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M. A., Noirhomme, Q., Lauwick, S., Luxen, A., Degueldre, C., Plenevaux, A., Schnakers, C., Phillips, C., Brichant, J. F., Bonhomme, V., Maquet, P., Greicius, M. D., Laureys, S. & Boly, M. (2010) Breakdown of within- and between-network resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. Anesthesiology 113:1038–53.
Bruns, A. & Eckhorn, R. (2004) Task-related coupling from high-to low-frequency signals among visual cortical areas in human subdural recordings. International Journal of Psychophysiology 51(2):97116.
Buzsáki, G. (2006) Rhythms of the brain. Oxford University Press.
Carlson, N. R. (1994) Physiology of behavior. Allyn and Bacon.
Cho, H., Zarolia, P., Gazzaley, A. & Morsella, E. (2016) Involuntary symbol manipulation (Pig Latin) from external control: Implications for thought suppression. Acta Psychologica 166:3741.
Cisek, P. (2007) Cortical mechanisms of action selection: The affordance competition hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 362:1585–99.
Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. (2010) Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annual Review of Neuroscience 33:269–98.
Clark, A. (2002) Is seeing all it seems? Action, reason and the grand illusion. Journal of Consciousness Studies 9:181202.
Cohen, M. A., Cavanagh, P., Chun, M. M. & Nakayama, K. (2012) The attentional requirements of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16:411–17.
Cooney, J. W. & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2003) Neurological disorders and the structure of human consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:161–66.
Cooper, A. D., Sterling, C. P., Bacon, M. P. & Bridgeman, B. (2012) Does action affect perception or memory? Vision Research 62:235–40.
Corr, P. J. & Morsella, E. (2015) The conscious control of behavior: Revisiting Gray's comparator model. In: Personality and control, vol. 4, ed. Corr, P. J., Fajkowska, M., Eysenck, M. W. & Wytykowska, A., pp. 1542. Eliot Werner.
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (1990) Toward a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences 2:263–75.
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (2000) The unconscious homunculus. In: Neural correlates of consciousness, ed. Metzinger, T., pp. 103–10. MIT Press.
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D. & Damasio, H. C. (1991) Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and preliminary testing. In: Frontal lobe function and dysfunction, ed. Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M. & Benton, A. L., pp. 217–29. Oxford University Press.
Dawkins, R. (1982) The extended phenotype: The long reach of the gene. Oxford University Press.
Dehaene, S. (2014) Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. Viking.
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown.
Desender, K., van Opstal, F. V. & van den Bussche, E. (2014) Feeling the conflict: The crucial role of conflict experience in adaptation. Psychological Science 25:675–83.
Doesburg, S. M., Green, J. L., McDonald, J. J. & Ward, L. M. (2009) Rhythms of consciousness: Binocular rivalry reveals large-scale oscillatory network dynamics mediating visual perception. PLoS ONE 4:e0006142.
Doesburg, S. M., Kitajo, K. & Ward, L. M. (2005) Increased gamma-band synchrony precedes switching of conscious perceptual objects in binocular rivalry. NeuroReport 16:1139–42.
Eeckman, F. H. & Freeman, W. J. (1990) Correlations between unit firing and EEG in the rat olfactory system. Brain Research 528(2):238–44.
Ehrsson, H. H. (2007) The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences. Science 317(5841):1048.
Eimer, M. & Schlaghecken, F. (2003) Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology 64(1):726.
Einstein, A. & Infeld, L. (1938/1967) The evolution of physics. Cambridge University Press/Touchstone. (Original work published in 1938).
Engel, A. K. & Singer, W. (2001) Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:1625.
Eriksen, C. W. & Schultz, D. W. (1979) Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Perception and Psychophysics 25:249–63.
Filevich, E. & Haggard, P. (2013) Persistence of internal representations of alternative voluntary actions. Frontiers in Psychology 4, article 202. (Online journal) doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00202.
Freeman, W. J. (1975) Mass action in the nervous system: Examination of the neurophysiological basis of adaptive behavior through the EEG. Academic Press.
Freeman, W. J. (1979) Nonlinear dynamics of paleocortex manifested in the olfactory EEG. Biological Cybernetics 35(1):2137.
Freeman, W. J. (1987) Nonlinear neural dynamics in olfaction as a model for cognition. In: Dynamics of sensory and cognitive processing in the brain, ed. Basar, E., pp. 1929. Springer-Verlag.
Freeman, W. J. (2004) William James on consciousness, revisited. Chaos and Complexity Letters 1:1742.
Freeman, W. J. (2007) Indirect biological measures of consciousness from field studies of brains as dynamical systems. Neural Networks 20:1021–31.
Fries, P. (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:474–80.
Frijda, N. H. (1986) The emotions. Cambridge University Press.
Frith, C. D. (2010) What is consciousness for? Pragmatics and Cognition 18(3):497551.
Glenn, L. L. & Dement, W. C. (1981) Membrane potential and input resistance of cat spinal motoneurons in wakefulness and sleep. Behavioural Brain Research 2:231–36.
Godwin, C. A., Gazzaley, A. & Morsella, E. (2013) Homing in on the brain mechanisms linked to consciousness: Buffer of the perception-and-action interface. In: The unity of mind, brain and world: Current perspectives on a science of consciousness, ed. Pereira, A. Jr. & Lehmann, D., pp. 4376. Cambridge University Press.
Goodale, M. & Milner, D. (2004) Sight unseen: An exploration of conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford University Press.
Gould, S. J. (1977) Ever since Darwin: Reflections in natural history. Norton.
Gray, C. M. & Skinner, J. E. (1988) Centrifugal regulation of neuronal activity in the olfactory bulb of the waking rabbit as revealed by reversible cryogenic blockade. Experimental Brain Research 69:378–86.
Grossberg, S. (1999) The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8(1):144.
Haidt, J. (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108:814–34.
Hallett, P. E. (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research 18:1279–96.
Hameroff, S. (2010) The “conscious pilot” – dendritic synchrony moves through the brain to mediate consciousness. Journal of Biological Physics 36:7193.
Harleß, E. (1861) Der Apparat des Willens [The apparatus of the will]. Zeitshrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik 38:499507.
Helmholtz, H. von (1856/1961) Treatise of physiological optics: Concerning the perceptions in general. In: Classics in psychology, ed. Shipley, T., pp. 79127. Philosophy Library. (Original work published in 1856).
Herz, R. S. (2003) The effect of verbal context on olfactory perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132:595606.
Hochberg, J. (1998) Gestalt theory and its legacy: Organization in eye and brain, in attention and mental representation. In: Perception and cognition at century's end: Handbook of perception and cognition, 2nd edition, ed. Hochberg, J., pp. 253306. Academic Press.
Hommel, B. (2013) Dancing in the dark: No role for consciousness in action control. Frontiers in Psychology 4, article 380. (Online journal). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00380.
Hong, L. E., Buchanan, R. W., Thaker, G. K., Shepard, P. D. & Summerfelt, A. (2008) Beta (~16 Hz) frequency neural oscillations mediate auditory sensory gating in humans. Psychophysiology 45(2):197204.
Hughes, G., Velmans, M. & de Fockert, J. (2009) Unconscious priming of a no-go response. Psychophysiology 46:1258–69.
Hummel, F. & Gerloff, C. (2005) Larger interregional synchrony is associated with greater behavioral success in a complex sensory integration task in humans. Cerebral Cortex 15:670–78.
Jackendoff, R. S. (1990) Consciousness and the computational mind. MIT Press.
Jackson, F. (1986) What Mary didn't know. The Journal of Philosophy 83:291–95.
James, W. (1890) The principles of psychology, vols. 1 & 2. Holt/Dover.
Jeannerod, M. (2006) Motor cognition: What action tells the self. Oxford University Press.
Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., Reber, P. J. & Kounios, J. (2004) Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biology 2:500–10.
Kaufman, M. T., Churchland, M. M., Ryu, S. I. & Shenoy, K. V. (2015) Vacillation, indecision and hesitation in moment-by-moment decoding of monkey cortex. eLife 4:e04677. doi:10.7554/eLife.04677.
Kay, L. M. & Beshel, J. (2010) A beta oscillation network in the rat olfactory system during a 2-alternative choice odor discrimination task. Journal of Neurophysiology 104:829–39.
Kay, L. M., Beshel, J., Brea, J., Martin, C., Rojas-L'bano, D. & Kopell, N. (2009) Olfactory oscillations: The what, how and what for. Trends in Neurosciences 32:207–14.
Kim, S., Singer, B. H. & Zochowski, M. (2006) Changing roles for temporal representation of odorant during the oscillatory response of the olfactory bulb. Neural Computation 18(4):794816.
Kinsbourne, M. (1996) What qualifies a representation for a role in consciousness? In: Scientific approaches to consciousness, ed. Cohen, J. D. & Schooler, J. W., pp. 335–55. Erlbaum.
Kinsbourne, M. (2000) How is consciousness expressed in the cerebral activation manifold? Brain and Mind 2:265–74.
Koch, C. (2004) The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Roberts.
Koch, C. & Tsuchiya, N. (2007) Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11:1622.
Koch, C. (2014) Consciousness. Colloquium delivered at the Redwood Center for Theoretical Neuroscience, December 2, 2014. University of California.
Krauzlis, R. J., Bollimunta, A., Arcizet, F. & Wang, L. (2014) Attention as an effect not a cause. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18:457–64.
Långsjö, J. W., Alkire, M. T., Kaskinoro, K., Hayama, H., Maksimow, A., Kaisti, K. K., Aalto, S., Aantaa, R., Jääskeläinen, S. K., Revonsuo, A. & Scheinin, H. (2012) Returning from oblivion: Imaging the neural core of consciousness. The Journal of Neuroscience 32:4935–43.
Lashley, K. S. (1956) Cerebral organization and behavior. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Diseases 36:118.
Laurent, G. & Davidowitz, H. (1994) Encoding of olfactory information with oscillating neural assemblies. Science 265(5180):1872–75.
LeDoux, J. E. (1996) The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon and Schuster.
LeDoux, J. E. (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23:155–84.
LeDoux, J. E. (2012) Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73:653–76.
Lee, U., Kim, S., Noh, G. J., Choi, B. M., Hwang, E. & Mashour, G. (2009) The directionality and functional organization of frontoparietal connectivity during consciousness and anesthesia in humans. Consciousness and Cognition 18:1069–78.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.
Lewis, L. D., Weiner, V. S., Mukamel, E. A., Donoghue, J. A., Eskandar, E. N., Madsen, J. R., Anderson, W., Hochberg, L. R., Cash, S. S., Brown, E. N. & Purdon, P. L. (2012) Rapid fragmentation of neuronal networks at the onset of propofol-induced unconsciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109(49):E3377–86.
Livnat, A. & Pippenger, N. (2006) An optimal brain can be composed of conflicting agents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103:3198–202.
Loewenstein, G. (1996) Out of control: visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 65:272–92.
Logan, G. D., Taylor, S. E. & Etherton, J. L. (1999) Attention and automaticity: Toward a theoretical integration. Psychological Research 62:165–81.
Logan, G. D., Yamaguchi, M., Schall, J. D. & Palmeri, T. J. (2015) Inhibitory control in mind and brain 2.0: Blocked-input models of saccadic countermanding. Psychological Review 122:115–47.
Logothetis, N. K. & Schall, J. D. (1989) Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science 245:761–62.
Lorenz, K. (1963) On aggression. Harcourt, Brace & World.
MacLeod, C. M. & MacDonald, P. A. (2000) Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4:383–91.
Macphail, E. M. (1998) The evolution of consciousness. Oxford University Press.
Mainland, J. D. & Sobel, N. (2006) The sniff is part of the olfactory percept. Chemical Senses 31:181–96.
Marcel, A. J. (1993) Slippage in the unity of consciousness. In: Experimental and theoretical studies of consciousness. Ciba Foundation Symposium 174, ed. Bock, G. R. & Marsh, J., pp. 168–80. Wiley.
Marr, D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman.
Martin, C., Beshel, J. & Kay, L. M. (2007) An olfacto-hippocampal network is dynamically involved in odor-discrimination learning. Journal of Neurophysiology 98(4):2196–205.
Mashour, G. A. (2004) Consciousness unbound: Toward a paradigm of general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 100:428–33.
Masicampo, E. J. & Baumeister, R. F. (2013) Conscious thought does not guide moment-to-moment actions – it serves social and cultural functions. Frontiers in Psychology 4, article 478. (Online journal). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00478.
Mayr, E. (2001) What evolution is. Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
McClelland, J. L. (1979) On the time-relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychological Review 86:287–30.
Meador, K. J., Ray, P. G., Echauz, J. R., Loring, D. W. & Vachtsevanos, G. J. (2002) Gamma coherence and conscious perception. Neurology 59:847–54.
Melzack, R. & Casey, K. L. (1968) Sensory, motivational, and central control determinants of pain: A new conceptual model. In: The skin senses, ed. Kenshalo, D. R., pp. 423–39. Charles C. Thomas.
Merker, B. (2007) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(1):6381; discussion 81–134.
Merker, B. (2012) From probabilities to percepts: A subcortical “global best estimate buffer” as locus of phenomenal experience. In: Being in time: Dynamical models of phenomenal experience, ed. Shimon, E., Tomer, F. & Zach, N., pp. 3780. John Benjamins.
Merker, B. (2013c) The efference cascade, consciousness, and its self: Naturalizing the first person pivot of action control. Frontiers in Psychology 4, article 501:120. (Online journal). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00501.
Merrick, C., Farnia, M., Jantz, T. K., Gazzaley, A. & Morsella, E. (2015) External control of the stream of consciousness: Stimulus-based effects on involuntary thought sequences. Consciousness and Cognition 33:217225.
Merrick, M. C., Godwin, C. A., Geisler, M. W. & Morsella, E. (2014) The olfactory system as the gateway to the neural correlates of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology 4, article1011. (Online journal). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01011.
Miller, N. E. (1959) Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learning. In: Psychology: A study of a science, vol. 2, ed. Koch, S., pp. 196292. McGraw-Hill.
Minsky, M. (1985) The society of mind. Simon and Schuster.
Molapour, T., Berger, C. C. & Morsella, E. (2011) Did I read or did I name? Process blindness from congruent processing “outputs.Consciousness and Cognition 20:1776–80.
Morsella, E. (2005) The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychological Review 112:1000–21.
Morsella, E. & Bargh, J. A. (2010b) What is an output? Psychological Inquiry 21:354–70.
Morsella, E. & Bargh, J. A. (2011) Unconscious action tendencies: Sources of “un-integrated” action. In: The handbook of social neuroscience, ed. Cacioppo, J. T. & Decety, J., pp. 335–47. Oxford University Press.
Morsella, E., Berger, C. C. & Krieger, S. C. (2011) Cognitive and neural components of the phenomenology of agency. Neurocase 17:209–30.
Morsella, E., Gray, J. R., Krieger, S. C. & Bargh, J. A. (2009a) The essence of conscious conflict: Subjective effects of sustaining incompatible intentions. Emotion 9:717–28.
Morsella, E., Wilson, L. E., Berger, C. C., Honhongva, M., Gazzaley, A. & Bargh, J. A. (2009c) Subjective aspects of cognitive control at different stages of processing. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics 71:1807–24.
Morsella, E., Zarolia, P. & Gazzaley, A. (2012) Cognitive conflict and consciousness. In: Cognitive consistency: A unifying concept in social psychology, ed. Gawronski, B. & Strack, F., pp. 1946. Guilford Press.
Mudrik, L., Faivre, N. & Koch, C. (2014) Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18(9):488–96.
Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T. & O'Reilly, R. C. (2011) A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15:453–59.
Nagasako, E. M., Oaklander, A. L. & Dworkin, R. H. (2003) Congenital insensitivity to pain: An update. Pain 101:213–19.
Nagel, T. (1974) What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 83:435–50.
Nath, A. R. & Beauchamp, M. S. (2012) A neural basis for interindividual differences in the McGurk effect, a multisensory speech illusion. NeuroImage 59:781–87.
Neumann, O. (1987) Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In: Perspectives on perception and action, ed. Heuer, H. & Sanders, A. F., pp. 361–94. Erlbaum.
Neville, K. R. & Haberly, L. B. (2003) Beta and gamma oscillations in the olfactory system of the urethane-anesthetized rat. Journal of Neurophysiology 90(6):3921–30.
Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977) Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84:231–59.
Oberauer, K. & Hein, L. (2012) Attention to information in working memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21:164–69.
Öhman, A. & Mineka, S. (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review 108:483522.
Olsson, A. & Phelps, E. A. (2004) Learned fear of “unseen” faces after Pavlovian, observational, and instructed fear. Psychological Science 15:822–28.
Panagiotaropolous, T. I., Deco, G., Kapoor, V. & Logothetis, N. K. (2012) Neuronal discharges and gamma oscillations explicitly reflect visual consciousness in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 74:924–35.
Pashler, H. (1993) Doing two things at the same time. American Scientist 81:4855.
Passingham, R. (1995) The frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford University Press.
Pinker, S. (1997) How the mind works. Norton.
Poellinger, A., Thomas, R., Lio, P., Lee, A., Makris, N., Rosen, B. R. & Kwong, K. K. (2001) Activation and habituation in olfaction-an fMRI study. Neuroimage 13(4):547–60.
Poehlman, T. A., Jantz, T. K. & Morsella, E. (2012) Adaptive skeletal muscle action requires anticipation and “conscious broadcasting.Frontiers in Cognition 3, article 369. (Online journal). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00369.
Preston, J. & Wegner, D. M. (2009) Elbow grease: The experience of effort in action. In: Oxford handbook of human action, ed. Morsella, E., Bargh, J. A. & Gollwitzer, P. M., pp. 469–86. Oxford University Press.
Prinz, J. (2007) The intermediate level theory of consciousness. In: The Blackwell companion to consciousness, ed. Velmans, M. & Schneider, S., pp. 248–60. Blackwell.
Prinz, W. (2003b) How do we know about our own actions? In: Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality, ed. Maasen, S., Prinz, W. & Roth, G., pp. 2133. Oxford University Press.
Prinz, W. (2012) Open minds: The social making of agency and intentionality. MIT Press.
Puttemans, V., Wenderoth, N. & Swinnen, S. P. (2005) Changes in brain activation during the acquisition of a multifrequency bimanual coordination task: From the cognitive stage to advanced levels of automaticity. Journal of Neuroscience 25:4270–78. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3866-04.2005
Ramachandran, V. S. (1999) Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind. Harper Perennial.
Riddle, T. A., Rosen, H. J. & Morsella, E. (2015) Is that me? Sense of agency as a function of intra-psychic conflict. Journal of Mind and Behavior 36:2746.
Roe, A. & Simpson, G. G. (1958) Behavior and evolution. Yale University Press.
Roser, M. & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2004) Automatic brains—interpretive minds. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13:5659.
Rosenbaum, D. A. (2002) Motor control. In: Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 1. Sensation and perception, 3rd edition, ed. Yantis, S., pp. 315–39. [Series editor: H. Pashler]. Wiley.
Sauseng, P. & Klimesch, W. (2008) What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32(5):1001–13.
Schlaghecken, F., Bowman, H. & Eimer, M. (2006) Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 32(3):618–32.
Schroter, M., Spoormaker, V., Schorer, A., Wohlschlager, A., Czish, M., Kochs, E., Zimmer, C., Hemmer, B., Schneider, G., Jordan, D. & Ilg, R. (2012) Spatiotemporal reconfiguration of large-scale brain functional networks during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. Journal of Neuroscience 32:12832–40.
Schrouff, J., Perlbarg, V., Boly, M., Marrelec, G., Boveroux, P., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M. A., Laureys, S., Phillips, C., Pélégrini-Isaac, M., Maquet, P. & Benali, H. (2011) Brain functional integration decreases during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. NeuroImage 57:198205.
Searle, J. R. (2000) Consciousness. Annual Review of Neurosciences 23:557–78.
Selfridge, O. G. (1959) Pandemonium: A paradigm for learning. In: Mechanization of thought processes: Proceedings of a Symposium held at the National Physics Laboratory, November 1958, vol. 1, ed. Blake, D. V. & Uttley, A. J.. pp. 511–26. Her Majesty's Stationary Office.
Sergent, C. & Dahaene, S. (2004) Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science 15:720–28.
Shallice, T. (1972) Dual functions of consciousness. Psychological Review 79:383–93.
Shepherd, G. M. (2006) Smell images and the flavour system in the human brain. Nature 444(7117):316–21.
Shepherd, G. M. (2007) Perspectives on olfactory processing, conscious perception, and orbitofrontal cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1121:87101.
Siegel, M., Donner, T. H. & Engel, A. K. (2012) Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13:121–34.
Simons, D. J. & Levin, D. T. (1997) Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1:261–67.
Simpson, G. G. (1949) The meaning of evolution. Yale University Press.
Singer, W. (2011) Consciousness and neuronal synchronization. In: The neurology of consciousness, ed. Laureys, S. & Tononi, G., pp. 4352. Academic Press.
Skinner, B. F. (1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan.
Sobel, N., Prabhakaran, V., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., Sullivan, E. V. & Gabrieli, J. D. (2000) Time course of odorant-induced activation in the human primary olfactory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 83(1):537–51.
Sperry, R. W. (1952) Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist 40:291312.
Stevenson, R. J. (2009) Phenomenal and access consciousness in olfaction. Consciousness and Cognition 18:1004–17. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.005.
Stopfer, M., Bhagavan, S., Smith, B. H. & Laurent, G. (1997) Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature 390(6655):7074.
Suhler, C. L. & Churchland, P. S. (2009) Control: Conscious and otherwise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13:341–47.
Tetlock, P. E. (2002) Social functionalist frameworks for judgment and choice: Intuitive politicians, theologians, and prosecutors. Psychological Review 109:451–71.
Thagard, P. & Stewart, T. C. (2014) Two theories of consciousness: Semantic pointer competition vs. information integration. Consciousness and Cognition 30:7390.
Thorndike, E. L. (1905) The functions of mental states. In: The elements of psychology, ed. Thorndike, E. L., pp. 111–19. A. G. Seiler.
Tipper, S. P. (1985) The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 37A:571–90.
Tolman, E. C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55:189208.
Tsotsos, J. K. (1995) Behaviorist intelligence and the scaling problem. Artificial Intelligence 75:135–60.
Tsotsos, J. K. (2011) A computational perspective on visual attention. MIT Press.
Uhlhaas, P. J., Pipa, G., Lima, B., Melloni, L., Neuenschwander, S., Nikolic, D. & Singer, W. (2009) Neural synchrony in cortical networks: History, concept and current status. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 3, article 17. (Online journal). doi:10.3389/neuro.07.017.2009.
Van Opstal, F., Gevers, W., Osman, M. & Verguts, T. (2010) Unconscious task application. Consciousness and Cognition 19:9991006.
Vanderwolf, C. H. & Zibrowski, E. M. (2001) Pyriform cortex beta-waves: Odor-specific sensitization following repeated olfactory stimulation. Brain Research 892:301308.
von Stein, A. & Sarnthein, J. (2000) Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: From local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology 38(3):301–13.
Ward, L. M. (2003) Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:553–59.
Welford, A. T. (1952) The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance: A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology 43:219.
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963) Symbol formation. Wiley.
Wessel, J. R., Haider, H. & Rose, M. (2012) The transition from implicit to explicit representations in incidental learning situations: More evidence from high-frequency EEG coupling. Experimental Brain Research 217:153–62.
Wilke, M., Mueller, K.-M. & Leopold, D. A. (2009) Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106:9465–70. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900714106.
Woodman, G. F. & Vogel, E. K. (2005) Fractionating working memory: Consolidation and maintenance are independent. Psychological Science 16:106113.
Wundt, W. (1902/1904) Principles of physiological psychology, trans. Titchener, E. B.. Sonnenschein. (Original work published in 1902; English translation from the 5th German edition [1904]).
Xu, F., Greer, C. A. & Shepherd, G. M. (2000) Odor maps in the olfactory bulb. Journal of Comparative Neurology 422(4):489–95.
Zibrowski, E. M. & Vanderwolf, C. H. (1997) Oscillatory fast wave activity in the rat pyriform cortex: Relations to olfaction and behavior. Brain Research 766:3949.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed