Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-d5ftd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-06T14:21:53.865Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Consumer Emotions

from Part I - Individual Consumer Decision Making and Behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2015

Michael I. Norton
Affiliation:
Harvard Business School, Harvard University
Derek D. Rucker
Affiliation:
Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University
Cait Lamberton
Affiliation:
Katz Graduate School of Business, University of Pittsburgh
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'

Information

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Book purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Aaker, D. A., Stayman, D. M., & Hagerty, M. R. (1986). Warmth in advertising – measurement, impact, and sequence effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(4), 365381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Buchanan, T. W. (2005). Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 512518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, C. T., Machleit, K. A., & Kleine, S. S. (1992). A comparison of attitudes and emotions as predictors of behavior at diverse levels of behavioral experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(4), 493504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E. B. (2005). Behavioral consequences of affect: Combining evaluative and regulatory mechanisms. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(3), 355362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E. B. & Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 9 (May), 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E. B. & Cohen, J. B. (2007a). Affect-based evaluation and regulation as mediators of behavior: The role of affect in risk-taking, helping and eating patterns. In Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Loewenstein, G. (eds), Do Emotions Help or Hurt Decision Making? A Hedgefoxian Perspective. New York: Russell Sage, 3568.Google Scholar
Andrade, E.B. & Cohen, J. B. (2007b). On the consumption of negative feelings. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(3), 283300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E. B. & Ho, T. H. (2009). Gaming emotions in social interactions. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(4), 539552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E. B. & Iyer, G. (2009). Planned versus actual betting in sequential gambles. Journal of Marketing Research, XLVI (June), 372383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E.B. & van Boven, L. (2010). Feelings not foregone: Underestimating affective reactions to what does not happen. Psychological Science, 21(5), 706711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apter, M. J. (1992). The Dangerous Edge: The Psychology of Excitement. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Arnold, M. J. & Reynolds, K. E. (2009). Affect and retail shopping behavior: Understanding the role of mood regulation and regulatory focus. Journal of Retailing, 85(3), 308320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atance, C. M. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2006). Preschoolers’ current desires warp their choice for the future. Psychology Science, 17(7), 583587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avnet, T., Pham, M. T. & Stephen, A. T. (2012). Consumers’ trust in feelings as information. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(4), 720735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, J. N. (1963). Induced moods and attitudes toward products. Journal of Advertising Research, 3(2), 1924.Google Scholar
Barone, M. J. Miniard, P. W., & Romero, J. B. (2000). The influence of positive mood on brand extension evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 386400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L.F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1(1), 2858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batra, R., & Ray, M. L. (1986). Affective responses mediating acceptance of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(2), 234249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batra, R., & Stayman, D. M. (1990). The role of mood in advertising effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(2), 203214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, H., Sujan, M., & Bettman, J. R. (1992). Autobiographical memories, affect, and consumer information processing. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1(1), 5382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2012). What makes online content viral? Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhatia, S., & Loewenstein, G. (2013). Drive states. In Diener, E. & Biswas-Diener, R. (eds.), Noba Textbook Series: Psychology. Champaign, IL: Diener Education Fund.Google Scholar
Bierley, C., McSweeney, F. K., & Vannieuwkerk, R. (1985). Classical conditioning of preference for stimuli. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(3), 316323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blaine, B. (2008). Does depression cause obesity? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies of depression and weight control. Journal of Health Psychology, 13(8), 11901197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory – a review. Psychological Bulletin, 99(2), 229246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blechert, J., Goltsche, J. E., Herbert, B. M., & Wilhelm, F. H. (2014). Eat your troubles away: Electrocortical and experiential correlates of food image processing are related to emotional eating style and emotional state. Biological Psychology, 96, 94101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodur, H. O., Brinberg, D., & Coupey, E. (2000). Belief, affect, and attitude: Alternative models of the determinants of attitude. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9(1), 1728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2000). Affect, memory, and social cognition. In Eich, E.K., Bower, G.H., Forgas, J.P., & Niedenthal, P.M. (eds.), Cognition and emotion (pp. 87-168). New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Bradley, C. M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Pictures as prepulse – attention and emotion in startle modification. Psychophysiology, 30(5), 541545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. L. (1992). Remembering pictures – pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 18(2), 379390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2003). The devaluation effect: Activating a need devalues unrelated choice options. Journal of Consumer Research. 29(4), 463473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. P., & Stayman, D. M. (1992). Antecedents and consequences of attitude toward the ad – a meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(1), 3451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. P., Homer, P. M., & Inman, J. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of relationships between ad-evoked feelings and advertising responses. Journal of Marketing Research, 35(1), 114126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecasting: The role of temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 14801493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, A., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (1992). Remembering emotional events. Memory & Cognition, 20(3), 277290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, M. C., & Edell, J. A. (1989). The impact of feelings on ad-based affect and cognition. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(1), 6983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, F., Tang, F., & Jia, J.-M. (2009). The interaction effect of mood and price level on purchase intention. In McGill, A. L. & Shavitt, S. (eds.), Advances in Consumer Research (vol. 36, pp. 963964). Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research.Google Scholar
Carmon, Z., Wertenbroch, K., & Zeelenberg, M. (2003). Option attachment: When deliberating makes choosing feel like losing. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 1529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, C., van Boven, L., Andrade, E. B., & Ariely, D. (2013). Moral violations reduce oral consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, online first, DOI: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandy, R. K., Tellis, G. J., MacInnis, D. J., Thaivanich, P. (2001). What to say when: Advertising appeals in evolving markets. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(4), 399414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, H. & Pham, M. T. (2013). Affect as a decision making system of the present. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(6), 4263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charles, S. T., Mather, M. & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 310324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chebat, J-C, & Michon, R. (2003). Impact of ambient odors on mall shoppers’ emotions, cognitions, and spending: A test of competitive causal theories. Journal of Business Research, 56, 529539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheema, A. and Patrick, V. (2012). Influence of warm versus cool temperatures on consumer choice: A resource depletion account. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(6), 984995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Darby, B. L., & Vincent, J. E. (1973). Transgression and altruism: Case for hedonism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9(6), 502516.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (1976). Altruism as hedonism: Social-development perspective on relationship of negative mood state and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(5), 907914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Cognitive Psychology (pp. 73108). New York: Elsevier/North-Holland.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. B., & Andrade, E. B. (2004). Affective intuition and task-contingent affect regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 358367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. B., & Areni, C. S. (1991). Affect and consumer behavior. In Robertson, T. S. & Kassarjian, H. H. (eds.), Handbook of Consumer Behavior (pp. 188240). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. B., Pham, M. T. & Andrade, E. B. (2008). The nature and role of affect in consumer behavior. In Haugtvedt, C., Kardes, F., & Mahway, P. Herr., Handbook of Consumer Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 297348.Google Scholar
Compton, R.J. (2003). The interface between emotion and attention: A review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2(2), 115129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constans, J. I., & Matthews, A. M. (1993). Mood and subjective risk of future events. Cognition and Emotion, 7(6), 545560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cote, S., Gyurak, A., & Levenson, R. W. (2010). The ability to regulate emotion is associated with greater well-being, income, and socioeconomic status. Emotion, 10(6), 923933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, D., Fischer, E., Johar, G., & Morwitz, V., (2014). From the editors-elect: Meaningful consumer research, Journal of Consumer Research, 41(1), iiiv.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahl, D. W. Manchanda, R. V, & Argo, J. J. (2001). Embarrassment in consumer purchase: The roles of social presence and purchase familiarity. Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (December), 473481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Wied, M., Zillmann, D., & Ordman, V. (1995). The role of empathic distress in the enjoyment of cinematic tragedy. Poetics. 23(1–2), 91106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Wegener, D. T., & Rucker, D. D. (2000). Beyond valence in the perception of likelihood: The role of emotion specificity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 397416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Muro, F. & Murray, K. B. (2012). An arousal regulation explanation of mood effects on consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(3), 574584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dommermuth, W. P., & Millard, W. J. (1967). Consumption coincidence in product evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 4, 388390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J. R. & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 736748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dutton, D. G. & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30(4), 510517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edell, J. A., & Burke, M. C. (1987). The power of feelings in understanding advertising effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 421433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmans, A., García, D., & Norli, O. (2007). Sports sentiment and stock returns. Journal of Finance, 62(4), 19671998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlichman, H., & Halpern, J. N. (1988). Affect and memory: Effects of pleasant and unpleasant odors on retrieval of happy and unhappy memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(5), 769779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eich, E. (1995). Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychological Science, 6(2), 6775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elster, J. (1999). Strong Feelings: Emotions, Addiction, and Human Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erber, M. W., & Erber, R. (2001). The role of motivated social cognition in the regulation of affective states. In Forgas, J. P. (ed.), Affect and Social Cognition (pp. 275289). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fedorikhin, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2010). Positive mood and resistance to temptation: The interfering influence of elevated arousal. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(4), 698711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 13(1), 117.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a mediator of emotion. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(3), 466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgas, J. P., Bower, G. H., & Krantz, S. E. (1984). The influence of mood on perceptions of social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 497513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 681700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frederickson, B. L. & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friestad, M. & Thorson, E. (1993). Remembering ads: The effects of encoding strategies, retrieval, and emotional response. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2(1), 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency. Cognition & Emotion, 1(2), 115143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galak, J., Krueger, J., & Loewenstein, G. (2013). Slow down! Insensitivity to rate of consumption leads to avoidable satiation. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(5), 9931009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garg, N., Wansink, B., & Inman, J. (2007). The influence of incidental affect on consumers’ food intake. Journal of Consumer Research, 71(1), 194206.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., & Blumberg, S. J., Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 617638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godden, D. & Baddeley, A. (1980). When does context influence recognition memory? British Journal of Psychology, 71(1), 99104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, M. E., & Gorn, G. J. (1987). Happy and sad TV programs: How they affect reactions to commercials. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 387403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorn, G. J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46(1), 94101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorn, G. J., Pham, M. T., & Sin, L. Y. (2001). When arousal influences ad evaluation and valence does not (and vice versa). Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(1), 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grasmick, H. G., Bursik, R., & Kinsey, K. A. (1991). Shame and embarrassment as deterrents to noncompliance with the law: The case of an antilittering campaign. Environment & Behavior, 23(2), 233251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, T. R., & Noice, H. (1988). Influence of positive affect upon creative-thinking and problem-solving in children. Psychological Reports, 63(3), 895898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., & Tsai, J. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology & Aging, 12(4), 590599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunberg, N. E., & Straub, R. O. (1992). The role of gender and taste class in the effects of stress on eating. Health Psychology, 11, 97100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemenover, S. H. & Schimmack, U. (2007). That´s disgusting!…But very amusing: Mixed feelings. Cognition & Emotion, 21(5), 11021113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirshleifer, D., & Shumway, T. (2003). Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. Journal of Finance, 58(3), 10091032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoch, S. J., & Loewenstein, G. F. (1991). Time-Inconsistent preferences and consumer self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(March), 492507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbrook, M. B., & Batra, R. (1987). Assessing the role of emotions as mediators of consumer responses to advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 404420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbrook, M. B. & Hirschman, E. C. (1982). The experiential aspects of consumption: Consumer fantasies, feelings, and fun. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(2), 132140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsee, C. K., & Rottenstreich, Y. (2004). Music, pandas, and muggers: On the affective psychology of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, X., Zhang, M., Hui, M. K., & Wyer, R. S. (2014). Warmth and conformity: The effects of ambient temperature on product preferences and financial decisions, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24, 241250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huntsinger, J.R. (2012). Does positive affect broaden and negative affect narrow attentional scope? New answer to an old question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 595600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. K. (eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (vol. 3, pp. 179236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of personality and social psychology, 47(6), 1206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Simmonds, S. F. (1978). The effect of feeling good on a helping task that is incompatible with good mood. Social Psychology, 41(4), 346349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem-solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 11221131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., Nygren, T. E., & Ashby, F. G. (1988). Influence of positive affect on the subjective utility of gains and losses: It is just not worth the risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(5), 710717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(1), 2031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (eds.), Well-being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology (pp. 325). New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
Kaltcheva, V. D. & Weitz, B. A. (2006). When should a retailer create an exciting store environment? Journal of Marketing, 70(January), 107118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keinan, A., & Kivetz, R. (2011). Productivity orientation and the consumption of collectable experiences. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 935950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. C., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 740752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenealy, P.M. (1997). Mood state-dependent retrieval: The effects of induced mood on memory reconsidered. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 290317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Effects of emotion on memory specificity in young and older adults. Journal of Gerontology B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62(4), 208215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H., Park, K., and Schwarz, N. (2010). Will this trip really be exciting? The role of incidental emotions in product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(6), 983991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., Allen, C. T., & Kardes, F. R. (1996). An investigation of the meditational mechanisms underlying attitudinal conditioning. Journal of Marketing Research, 33(3), 318328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krishna, A. (2012). An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 332351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krishna, A., Morrin, M., & Sayin, E. (2014). Smellizing cookies and salivating: A focus on olfactory imagery. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(April), doi:10.1086/674664.Google Scholar
Krishna, A., & Schwarz, N. (2014). Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A review and introduction. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 159168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaBar, K. S., Mesulam, M.-M., Gitelman, D. R., & Weintraud, S. (2000). Emotional curiosity: Modulation of visuospatial attention by arousal is preserved in aging and early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 38(13), 17341740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labroo, A. A., & Mukhopadhyay, A. (2009). Lay theories of emotion transience and the search for happiness: A fresh perspective on affect regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(2), 242254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labroo, A. A., & Patrick, V. M. (2009). Psychological distancing: Why happiness helps you see the big picture. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 800809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labroo, A. A., & Rucker, D. D. (2010). The orientation-matching hypothesis: An emotion-specificity approach to affect regulation. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(5), 955966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, J. T., McGraw, P. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 684696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lau-Gesk, L., & Drolet, A. (2008). The publicly self-consciousness consumer: Prepared to be embarrassed. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18 (2), 127136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawson, R. (1985). The effects of mood on retrieving consumer product information. In Hirschman, E. C. & Holbrook, M. B. (eds.), Advances in Consumer Research (vol. 12, pp. 399403). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.Google Scholar
Lee, A. Y., & Sternthal, B. (1999). The effects of positive mood on memory. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(2), 115127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. J. & Andrade, E. B. (2011). Fear, social projection, and financial decision making. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(SPL), S121S129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. J., Andrade, E. B., and Palmer, S. E. (2013). Interpersonal relationships and preferences for mood-congruency in aesthetic experiences. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(August), 382391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, L., & Tsai, C. (2014). How price promotions influence postpurchase consumption experience over time. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(5), 943959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 81(1), 146159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, L. J., & Safer, M. A. (2002). Sources of bias in memory for emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 169173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, L. J., Lench, H. C., Kaplan, R. L., & Safer, M.A. (2012). Accuracy and artifact: Reexamining the intensity bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(4), 584605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, L. J., Lench, H. C., Kaplan, R. L., & Safer, M. A. (2013). Like Schrödinger’s cat, the impact bias is both dead and alive: Reply to Wilson and Gilbert (2013). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,105(5), 749756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(3), 272292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (1999) Because it is there: The challenge of mountaineering… for utility theory. Kyklos, 52, 315344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (2000). Emotions in economic theory and economic behavior. American Economic Review: Papers and Proceedings, 90, 426432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, M. F. (1998). Choosing to avoid: Coping with negatively emotion-laden consumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 409433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, M. F., Payne, J. W., & Bettman, J. R. (1999). Emotional trade-off difficulty and choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 36(2), 143159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacGregor, D. G., Slovic, P., Dreman, D., & Berry, M. (2000). Imagery, affect, and financial judgment. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 1(2), 104110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machleit, K. A., & Wilson, R. D. (1988). Emotional feelings and attitudes toward the advertisement: The roles of brand familiarity and repetition. Journal of Advertising, 17(3), 2735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, L. L., Abend, T., Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (1997). How would it feel if…? Mood as input to a role fulfillment evaluation process. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 73(2), 242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, M., Gorlick, M. A., & Nesmith, K. (2009). The limits of arousal’s memory impairing effects on nearby information. American Journal of Psychology, 122, 349370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 3352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., & Johnson, M. K. (2003). Affective review and schema reliance in memory in older and younger adults. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 169189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., & Nesmith, K. (2008). Arousal-enhanced location memory for pictures. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 449464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. (2009). Disentangling the effects of arousal and valence on memory for intrinsic details. Emotion Review, 1, 118119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 114133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braveman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(1), 119132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAlister, L. (1982). A dynamic attribute satiation model of variety-seeking behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(2), 141150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGraw, A. P. & Warren, C. (2010). Benign violations: Making immoral behavior funny. Psychological Science, 21, 11411149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGraw, A. P., Warren, C., Williams, L., & Leonard, B. (2012). Too close for comfort, or too far to care? Finding humor in distant tragedies and close mishaps. Psychological Science, 25, 12151223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., & Ritov, I. (1999). Emotion-based choice. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: General, 128(3), 332345.Google Scholar
Meyvis, T., Ratner, R. and Levav, J. (2010). Why don’t we learn to accurately forecast feelings? How misremembering our predictions blinds us to past forecasting errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(November), 579589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, T.R., Thompson, L., Peterson, E. & Cronk, R. (1997). Temporal adjustments in the evaluation of events: The “rosy view.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(4), 421448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mogilner, C., Aaker, J., & Kamvar, S. (2012). How happiness affects choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 429443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, A. C., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2007). Product contagion: Changing consumer evaluations through physical contact with “disgusting” products. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(2), 272283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, A. C., Wu, E. C., and Fitzsimons, G. J. (2012). How disgust enhances the effectiveness of fear appeals. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(3), 383393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moriya, H., & Nittono, H. (2011). Effect of mood states on the breadth of spatial attentional focus: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 11621170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2000). How emotions work: An analysis of the social functions of emotional expression in negotiations. Review of Organizational Behavior, 22, 150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The Frame of Mind. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, L. D., & Meyvis, T. (2008). Interrupted consumption: Adaptation and the disruption of hedonic experience. Journal of Marketing Research, 45, 654664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, J. H., Shapiro, S. A., & Mason, C. H. (2010). Emotionality and semantic onsets: Exploring orienting attention responses in advertising. Journal of Marketing Research: December, 47(6), 11381150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism, Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliver, R. (1993). Cognitive, affective, and attribute bases of the satisfaction response. Journal of Consumer Research, 20(December), 418430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottati, V. C., & Isbell, L. M. (1996). Effects of mood during exposure to target information on subsequently reported judgments: An on-line model of misattribution and correction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 3953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, V. M., MacInnis, D. J., & Park, C. W. (2007). Not as happy as I thought I’d be? Affective misforecasting and product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4), 479489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pham, M. T. (1996). Cue representation and selection effects of arousal on persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 22(4), 373387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pham, M. T. (1998). Representativeness, relevance, and the use of feelings in decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(2), 144159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pham, M.T. (2013). The seven sins of consumer psychology. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23 (Oct), 411423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pham, M. T., Cohen, J. B., Pracejus, J. W., & Hughes, G. D. (2001). Affect monitoring and the primacy of feelings in judgment. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(2), 167188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pham, M. T., Gorn, G., & Sin, L. Y. (2001). When arousal influences ad evaluation and valence does not (and vice versa). Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(1), 4355.Google Scholar
Pietromonaco, P. R., & Rook, K. S. (1987). Decision style in depression: The contribution of perceived risks versus benefits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(2), 399408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pocheptsova, A., & Novemsky, N. (2010). When do incidental mood effects last? Lay beliefs versus actual effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(6), 9921001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richins, M. L. (1983). An analysis of consumer interaction styles in the marketplace. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(1), 7382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological bulletin, 128(6), 934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. Review of Personality & Social Psychology, 5, 1136.Google Scholar
Rosler, A., Ulrich, C., Billino, J., Sterzer, P., Weidauer, S., Bernhardt, T., Steinmetz, H., Frolich, L., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2005). Effects of arousing emotional scenes on the distribution of visuospatial attention: Changes with aging and early subcortical vascular dementia. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 229–30 (March 15), 109116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K., (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(1), 383388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. (1990). Getting to like the burn of chili pepper: Biological, psychological and cultural perspectives. In Green, B. G., Mason, J. R., & Kare, M. R. (eds.), Chemical Senses (vol. 2, Irritation; pp. 231269). New York: Marcel Dekker.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Guillot, L., Fincher, K., Rozin, A., & Tsukayama, E. (2013). Glad to be sad, and other examples of benign masochism. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(4), 439447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 805819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C.L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 165196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rusting, C. L., & DeHart, T. (2000). Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: Consequences for mood-congruent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 737752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakaki, M., Fryer, K., & Mather, M. (2014). Emotion strengthens high priority memory traces but weakens low priority memory traces. Psychological Science, 25(2), 387395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salermo, A., Laran, J., & Janiszewski, C. (2014). Hedonic eating goals and emotion: When sadness decreases the desire to indulge. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(June), 135151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanbonmatsu, D. M., & Kardes, F. R. (1988). The effects of physiological arousal on information-processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3), 379385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962) Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(September), 379399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (1982). Emotion as a process: Function, origin, and regulation. Social Science Information, 21(4–5), 555570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schimmack, U. (2001). Pleasure, displeasure, and mixed feelings? Are semantic opposites mutually exclusive? Cognition and Emotion, 15, 8197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schindler, R.M. (1998). Consequences of perceiving oneself as responsible for obtaining a discount: Evidence for smart-shopper feelings. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7(4), 371392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schramm, H., & Wirth, W. (2010). Exploring the paradox of sad-film enjoyment: The role of multiple appraisals and meta-appraisals. Poetics, 38, 319335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Sorrentino, R. M. & Higgins, E. T. (eds.), Handbook of Motivation and Cognition (vol. 2, pp. 521561). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (2001). Feelings as information: Implications for affective influences on information processing. In Martin, L. L. & Clore, G. (eds.), Theories of Mood and Cognition: A User’s Guidebook (vol. 8, pp. 159172). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Kruglanski, A. & Higgins, E. T. (eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (2nd ed., pp. 385407). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C. (1994). Incongruent effects of sad mood on self-conception valence: It’s a matter of time. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 161172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shen, H., & Wyer, R.S. (2008). Procedural priming and consumer judgments: Effects on the impact of positively and negatively valenced information. Journal Consumer Research, 34(5), 727737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheth, J. N. (1982). Consumer behavior: Surpluses and shortages. Advances in Consumer Research (vol. 19, pp. 1316), Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.Google Scholar
Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P. (2010). The Feeling of Risk: New Perspectives on Risk Perception. New York: Earthscan.Google Scholar
Small, D. & Verocchi, N. M. (2009). The face of need: Facial emotion expression on charity advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(6), 777787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. D., Wilson, R. J., & Jones, B. E. (1983). Extraversion and multiple levels of caffeine-induced arousal: Effects on overhabituation and dishabituation. Psychophysiology, 20(1), 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. A., and Ellsworth, P. C. (1985), Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soman, D. & Cheema, A. (2011). Earmarking and partitioning: increasing saving by low-income households. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(SPL), S14S22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangenberg, E. R., Crowley, A. E., & Henderson, P. W. (1996). Improving the store environment: Do olfactory cues affect evaluations and behaviors? Journal of Marketing, 60(2), 6780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stayman, D. M. & Batra, R. (1991). Encoding and retrieval of ad affect in memory. Journal of Marketing Research, 28(2), 232239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steblay, N. M. (1992). A meta-analytic review of the weapon focus effect. Law and Human Behavior, 16(4), 413424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, D. N., & Kadous, K. (1997). The joint effects of task-related negative affect and task difficulty in multiattribute choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(2), 159174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, M. R., & Mather, M. (2012). Negative arousal amplifies the effects of saliency in short-term memory. Emotion, 12, 13671372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tal, A., & Wansink, B. (2013). Fattening fasting: Hungry grocery shoppers buy more calories, not more food. JAMA Internal Medicine, 173(12), 11461148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Pleasure and utility in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 101105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teixeira, T., Wedel, M., & Pieters, R. (2012). Emotion-induced engagement in internet video advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 144159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, M., MacInnis, D. J., & Park, C. W. (2005). The ties that bind: Measuring the strength of consumers’ emotional attachments to brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(1), 7791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorson, E., & Friestad, M. (1989). The effects of emotion on episodic memory for television commercials. In Cafferata, P. & Tybout, A. M. (eds.), Cognitive and Affective Responses to Advertising (pp. 305325). Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath and Company.Google Scholar
Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 5367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(2), 288307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bergh, B., Dewitte, S., and Warlop, L. (2008). Bikinis instigate generalized impatience in intertemporal choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 8597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vosgerau, J. (2010). How prevalent is wishful thinking? Misattribution of arousal causes optimism and pessimism in subjective probabilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 3248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J., Novemsky, N., & Dhar, R. (2009). Anticipating adaptation to products. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(August), 149159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wansink, B., & Chandon, P. (2014). Slim by design: Redirecting the accidental drivers of mindless overeating. Journal of Consumer Psychology, DOI: 10.1016/j.jcps.2014.03.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wansink, B., Payne, C. R., & Chandon, P. (2007). Internal and external cues of meal cessarion: The French paradox redux? Obesity, 15(12), 29202924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B., Tal, A., & Shimizu, M. (2012). First foods most: After 18-hour fast, people drawn to starches first and vegetables last, JAMA Internal Medicine, 172(12), 961963.Google ScholarPubMed
Ward, M. H., & Dahl, D. W. (2104). Should the devil Sell Prada? Retail rejection increases aspiring consumers’ desire for the brand. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(3), 590-609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Community noise problems: Evidence against adaptation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2(2), 8797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, P., & Aaker, J. L. (2002). Can mixed emotions peacefully coexist? Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 636649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In Zanna, M. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (vol. 35, pp. 345411). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 821836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winkielman, P, & Berridge, K. 2004. Unconscious emotion. Current Directions Psychological Science, 13, 120123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wirth, W., Hofer, M., & Schramm, H. (2012). Beyond pleasure: Exploring the eudaimonic entertainment experience. Human Communication Research, 38(4), 406428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2003).What to do on spring break? The role of predicted, on-line, and remembered experience in future choice. Psychological Science, 14(5), 520524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, S. L., & Bettman, R. B. (2007). Predicting happiness: How normative feeling rules influence (and even reverse) durability bias. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(3), 188201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, W. F., & Bower, G. H. (1982). Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organizational Behavioral and Human Decision Processes, 52(2), 276291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyer, R. S., & Carlston, D. (1979). Social Cognition, Inference and Attribution. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Xu, J., & Schwarz, N. (2009). Do we really need a reason to indulge? Journal of Marketing Research, 46(1), 2536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeelenberg, R., & Bocanegra, B. R. (2010). Auditory emotional cues enhance visual perception. Cognition, 115(1), 202206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management through communication choices. American Behavioral Scientist, 31(3), 327340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwebner, Y., Lee, L., & Goldenberg, J. (2013). The temperature premium: Warm temperatures increase product valuation. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 251259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Accessibility standard: Unknown

Accessibility compliance for the PDF of this book is currently unknown and may be updated in the future.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×