Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-08T21:47:53.245Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multisensory development

from Part IV - Perceptual and cognitive development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2017

Brian Hopkins
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Elena Geangu
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Sally Linkenauger
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Further reading

Bremner, A.J., & Spence, C. (2017). The development of touch perception. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 52, 227–268.Google Scholar
Burr, D., Binda, P., & Gori, M. (2011). Combinding vision with audition and touch, in adults and children. In J. Trommershauser, K. Kording, & M. S. Landy (Eds.), Sensory cue integration (pp. 173–194). Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Murray, M.M., Lewkowicz, D.J., Amedi, A., & Wallace, M.T. (2016). Multisensory processes: A balancing act across the lifespan. Trends in Neurosciences, 39, 567–579.Google Scholar
Trommershauser, J., Kording, K., & Landy, M. S. (Eds.). (2011). Sensory cue integration. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar

Acknowledgments

The writing of this entry was partially funded by the European Research Council (FP7/2007–2013) (Grant agreement no. 241242).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L.E., & Lickliter, R. (2012). The role of intersensory redundancy in early perceptual, cognitive, and social development. In Bremner, A.J., Lewkowicz, D.J., & Spence, C. (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. 183206). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L.E., & Lickliter, R. (2014). Learning to attend selectively: The dual role of intersensory redundancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 414420.Google Scholar
Begum Ali, J., Spence, C., & Bremner, A.J. (2015). Human infants’ ability to perceive touch in external space develops postnatally. Current Biology, 25, R978R979.Google Scholar
Birch, H.G., & Lefford, A. (1963). Intersensory development in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 28, 148.Google Scholar
Bremner, A.J. (2016). Developing body representations in early life: Combining somatosensation and vision to perceive the interface between the body and the world. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 58, 1216.Google Scholar
Bremner, A.J., & de Fockert, J.W. (2016). Sensory development: Childhood changes in visual cortical function. Current Biology, 26, R36R37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, A.J., Lewkowicz, D.J., & Spence, C. (2012). Multisensory development. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowie, D., Makin, T., & Bremner, A.J. (2013). Children’s responses to the Rubber Hand Illusion reveal dissociable pathways in body representations. Psychological Science, 24, 762769.Google Scholar
Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Are we all born synaesthetic? Examining the neonatal synaesthesia hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 12401253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M.O., & Banks, M.S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature, 415, 429433.Google Scholar
Gibson, E.J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. East Norfolk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Gori, M., Del Viva, M.M., Sandini, G., & Burr, D.C. (2008). Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information. Current Biology, 18, 694698.Google Scholar
Hill, E.L., Crane, L., & Bremner, A.J. (2012). Developmental disorders and multisensory perception. In Bremner, A.J., Lewkowicz, D.J., & Spence, C. (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. 273300). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D.J. (2000). The development of intersensory temporal perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations view. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 281308.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D.J., & Ghazanfar, A.A. (2009). The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 470478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurer, D., Gibson, L.C., & Spector, F. (2012). Infant synaesthesia: New insights into the development of multisensory perception. In Bremner, A.J., Lewkowicz, D.J., & Spence, C. (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. 229250). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nardini, M., Jones, P., Bedford, R., & Braddick, O. (2008). Development of cue integration in human navigation. Current Biology, 18, 689693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rigato, S., Begum Ali, J., van Velzen, J., & Bremner, A.J. (2014). The neural basis of somatosensory remapping develops in human infancy. Current Biology, 24, 12221226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaal, B., & Durand, K. (2012). The role of olfaction in human multisensory development. In Bremner, A.J., Lewkowicz, D.J., & Spence, C. (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. 2962). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, B. (Ed.). (2012). The new handbook of multisensory processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Streri, A. (2012). Crossmodal interactions in the human newborn: New answers to Molyneux’s question. In Bremner, A.J., Lewkowicz, D.J., & Spence, C. (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. 88112). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taga, G., & Asakawa, K. (2007). Selectivity and localization of cortical response to auditory and visual stimulation in awake infants aged 2 to 4 months. NeuroImage, 36, 12461252.Google Scholar
Walker, P., Bremner, J.G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., & Johnson, S.P. (2014). Preverbal infants are sensitive to cross-sensory correspondences: Much ado about the null results of Lewkowicz and Minar (2014). Psychological Science, 25, 835836.Google Scholar
Wolff, P.H., Matsumiya, Y., Abroms, I.F., Van Velzer, C., & Lombroso, C.T. (1974). The effect of white noise on the somatosensory evoked response in sleeping newborn infants. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 37, 269274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×