Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-16T22:41:59.125Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infants aren't biased toward fearful faces

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Andrew M. Herbert
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY 14623, USA amhgss@rit.edu, kxcgsh@rit.edu, tmsgsh@rit.edu https://people.rit.edu/amhgss/, https://www.rit.edu/liberalarts/directory/kxcgsh-kirsten-condry, https://www.rit.edu/liberalarts/directory/tmsgsh-tina-sutton
Kirsten Condry
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY 14623, USA amhgss@rit.edu, kxcgsh@rit.edu, tmsgsh@rit.edu https://people.rit.edu/amhgss/, https://www.rit.edu/liberalarts/directory/kxcgsh-kirsten-condry, https://www.rit.edu/liberalarts/directory/tmsgsh-tina-sutton
Tina M. Sutton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY 14623, USA amhgss@rit.edu, kxcgsh@rit.edu, tmsgsh@rit.edu https://people.rit.edu/amhgss/, https://www.rit.edu/liberalarts/directory/kxcgsh-kirsten-condry, https://www.rit.edu/liberalarts/directory/tmsgsh-tina-sutton

Abstract

Grossmann's argument for the “fearful ape hypothesis” rests on an incomplete review of infant responses to emotional faces. An alternate interpretation of the literature argues the opposite, that an early preference for happy faces predicts cooperative learning. Questions remain as to whether infants can interpret affect from faces, limiting the conclusion that any “fear bias” means the infant is fearful.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2010). The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the affective Simon task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(3), 310328. doi:10.1177/1367006910379261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrera, M. E., & Maurer, D. (1981). The perception of facial expressions by the three-month-old. Child Development, 52(1), 203206. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beall, P. M., & Herbert, A. M. (2008). The face wins: Stronger automatic processing of affect in facial expressions than words in a modified Stroop task. Cognition and Emotion, 22(8), 16131642. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930801940370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosch, T., Pourtois, G., & Sander, D. (2010). The perception and categorisation of emotional stimuli: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 24(3), 377400. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902975754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvo, M. G., & Lundqvist, D. (2008). Facial expressions of emotion (KDEF): Identification under different display-duration conditions. Behavior Research Methods, 40(1), 109115. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.1.109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2008). Detection of emotional faces: Salient physical features guide effective visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(3), 471494. doi:10.1037/a0012771CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coelho, C. M., Cloete, S., & Wallis, G. (2010). The face-in-the-crowd effect: When angry faces are just cross(es). Journal of Vision, 10(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.1.7Google ScholarPubMed
Craig, B. M., Becker, S. I., & Lipp, O. V. (2014). Different faces in the crowd: A happiness superiority effect for schematic faces in heterogeneous backgrounds. Emotion, 14(4), 794803. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durand, K., Gallay, M., Seigneuric, A., Robichon, F., & Baudouin, J. (2007). The development of facial emotion recognition: The role of configural information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(1), 1427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farroni, T., Menon, E., Rigato, S., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). The perception of facial expressions in newborns. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(1), 213. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620601046832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179181. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7123230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, X, & Maurer, D. (2009). Influence of intensity on children's sensitivity to happy, sad, and fearful facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 503521. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, X., & Maurer, D. (2010). A happy story: Developmental changes in children's sensitivity to facial expressions of varying intensities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 6786. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Neural signatures of conscious and unconscious emotional face processing in human infants. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 64, 260270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kataja, E., Leppänen, J. M., Kantojärvi, K., Pelto, J., Häikiö, T., Korja, R., … Karlsson, L. (2020). The role of TPH2 variant rs4570625 in shaping infant attention to social signals. Infant Behavior & Development, 60, 12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kauschke, C., Bahn, D., Vesker, M., & Schwarzer, G. (2019). The role of emotional valence for the processing of facial and verbal stimuli – Positivity or negativity bias? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01654CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotsoni, E., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2001). Categorical perception of facial expressions by 7-month-old infants. Perception, 30(9), 11151125. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(3), 381396. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peltola, M. J., Hietanen, J. K., Forssman, L., & Leppänen, J. M. (2013). The emergence and stability of the attentional bias to fearful faces in infancy. Infancy, 18(6), 905926. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peltola, M. J., Leppänen, J. M., Palokangas, T., & Hietanen, J. K. (2008). Fearful faces modulate looking duration and attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 11(1), 6068. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00659.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, D. G., & Stewart, A. L. (2010). Still another confounded face in the crowd. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(8), 21152127. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.8.2115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, D. G., Stewart, A. L., & Skov, R. B. (1996). It takes a confounded face to pop out of a crowd. Perception, 25(9), 10911108. https://doi.org/10.1068/p251091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, P. C., Anzures, G., Izard, C. E., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Tanaka, J. W. (2011). Looking across domains to understand infant representation of emotion. Emotion Review, 3(2), 197206. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910387941CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruba, A. L., & Repacholi, B. M. (2020). Do preverbal infants understand discrete facial expressions of emotion? Emotion Review, 12(4), 235250. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073919871098CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, R. A., Lipp, O. V., Craig, B. M., Becker, S. I., & Horstmann, G. (2013). In search of the emotional face: Anger versus happiness superiority in visual search. Emotion, 13(4), 758768. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031970CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serrano, J. M., Iglesias, J., & Loeches, A. (1992). Visual discrimination and recognition of facial expressions of anger, fear, and surprise in 4- to 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 25(6), 411425. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420250603CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, T. M., & Lutz, C. (2019). Attentional capture for emotional words and images: The importance of valence and arousal. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 73(1), 4754. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vesker, M., Bahn, D., Degé, F., Kauschke, C., & Schwarzer, G. (2018). Developmental changes in the categorical processing of positive and negative facial expressions. PLoS ONE, 13(8), 13. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201521CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, J. B., Keehn, B., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2020). Attentional bias to fearful faces in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder. Emotion, 20(6), 980992. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker-Andrews, A. (1997). Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 437456. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.437CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., Kim, H., Polis, S., … Johnstone, T. (2004). Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science (New York, N.Y.), 306(5704), 2061. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103617CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. M., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 324. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, W., McCormick, S. A., Westerlund, A., Bowman, L. C., & Nelson, C. A. (2019). Neural correlates of facial emotion processing in infancy. Developmental Science, 22(3), 116. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12758CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 347. doi:10.1080/02699930903205698CrossRefGoogle Scholar