We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Recent changes in US government priorities have serious negative implications for science that will compromise the integrity of mental health research, which focuses on vulnerable populations. Therefore, as editors of mental science journals and custodians of the academic record, we confirm with conviction our collective commitment to communicating the truth.
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a serious and often chronic illness that requires early and urgent treatment. Failing to provide effective treatment of MDD can worsen the illness trajectory, negatively impact physical health, and even alter brain structure. Early optimized treatment (EOT) of MDD, with a measurement-based approach to diagnosis, rapid treatment initiation with medication dosage optimization, frequent monitoring, and prompt adjustments in treatment planning when indicated, should proceed with a sense of urgency. In this article, we describe common barriers to providing an EOT approach to treating MDD at each phase of care, along with strategies for navigating these obstacles. Approaching the treatment of MDD with a greater sense of urgency increases the likelihood of symptom reduction in MDD, facilitating full functional recovery and a return to life engagement.
Objectives/Goals: Depression is common among people living with HIV (PLWH). This study explored the link between reduced metacognitive awareness and depression in PLWH. It utilized a positive emotion regulation task to compare brain activation during viewing versus upregulating positive emotions. Methods/Study Population: Depressed PLWH (N = 24; mean age = 53; HAM-D mean = 19) participated in an emotion regulation task while blood oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) responses were recorded. In the emotional regulation task, participants were shown the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) a series of positive, negative, and neutral images. Participants were asked to view these images and given instructions to either negatively reappraise (RN) or positively reappraise (RP). In the RP condition, participants were no longer shown the image and asked to upregulate their positive emotional responses associated with it. Ten onset times were included for each trial. Results/Anticipated Results: A one-sample t-test was conducted to analyze contrasts between reappraisal of positive images and viewing positive images (RP > VP). Results showed significantly greater activation in the posterior cingulate and angular gyrus during the RP condition (peak MNI: 18, -52, 34; p < 0.001, uncorrected, k > 10 voxels). In comparing the reappraisal of negative images to viewing negative images (RN > VN), there was increased activation in the right supramarginal gyrus (peak MNI: 50, -28, 22; p < 0.001, uncorrected, k > 10 voxels). When contrasting the reappraisal of positive to negative images (RP > RN), BOLD signals were higher in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (peak MNI: 40, -38, 32; p < 0.001, uncorrected, k > 10 voxels). Discussion/Significance of Impact: Findings underscore that depressed PLWH demonstrates BOLD responses in brain regions linked to appetitive motivation and meta-cognitive awareness during the RP condition which demands more executive resources among those with depression, highlighting the complexity of emotional regulation in this population.
Internal and external rotation of the shoulder is often challenging to quantify in the clinic. Existing technologies, such as motion capture, can be expensive or require significant time to setup, collect data, and process and analyze the data. Other methods may rely on surveys or analog tools, which are subject to interpretation. The current study evaluates a novel, engineered, wearable sensor system for improved internal and external shoulder rotation monitoring, and applies it in healthy individuals. Using the design principles of the Japanese art of kirigami (folding and cutting of paper to design 3D shapes), the sensor platform conforms to the shape of the shoulder with four on-board strain gauges to measure movement. Our objective was to examine how well this kirigami-inspired shoulder patch could identify differences in shoulder kinematics between internal and external rotation as individuals moved their humerus through movement patterns defined by Codman’s paradox. Seventeen participants donned the sensor while the strain gauges measured skin deformation patterns during the participants’ movement. One-dimensional statistical parametric mapping explored differences in strain voltage between the rotations. The sensor detected distinct differences between the internal and external shoulder rotation movements. Three of the four strain gauges detected significant temporal differences between internal and external rotation (all p < .047), particularly for the strain gauges placed distal or posterior to the acromion. These results are clinically significant, as they suggest a new class of wearable sensors conforming to the shoulder can measure differences in skin surface deformation corresponding to the underlying humerus rotation.
Daqingshanite in the Kamthai REE deposit (India) occurs as two paragenetic types: primary granular coarse grained crystals coexisting with primary carbocernaite, baryte and bastnäsite; and as aligned micro-ovoid globules within clasts of Sr-bearing calcite. Carbocernaite forming trellis-type lamellae in some of these calcite clasts do not represent exsolution and are considered as replacement textures as they formed subsequent to daqingshanite. The origins of the textural relations of the microglobules of daqingshanite to their host Sr-calcite cannot be unambiguously determined, although an exsolution origin is not considered feasible. The textures are similar to those of ‘chalcopyrite disease’ and as such could be interpreted as replacement features formed in a low temperature carbothermal environment which should facilitate replacement. Given that daqingshanite is an early crystallising phase it is also possible that cotectic crystallisation with Sr-calcite occurred, followed by subsolidus re-equilibration with recrystallisation along specific crystallographic planes in the calcite. The Kamthai REE deposit is best described as a low temperature carbothermalite microbreccia consisting of a wide variety of clasts resulting from the autobrecciation of rocks formed during, and after, the magmatic to carbothermal transition of an undetermined parental calcite carbonatite-forming magma. Many clasts have been replaced by late stage La-enriched carbothermal fluids mixed with exogenous water during the final low-temperature stage of evolution of the deposit.
Contrast-induced encephalopathy (CIE) is an adverse event associated with diagnostic and therapeutic endovascular procedures. Decades of animal and human research support a mechanistic role for pathological blood-brain barrier dysfunction (BBBd). Here, we describe an institutional case series and review the literature supporting a mechanistic role for BBBd in CIE.
Methods:
A literature review was conducted by searching MEDLINE, Web of Science, Embase, CINAHL and Cochrane databases from inception to January 31, 2022. We searched our institutional neurovascular database for cases of CIE following endovascular treatment of cerebrovascular disease during a 6-month period. Informed consent was obtained in all cases.
Results:
Review of the literature revealed risk factors for BBBd and CIE, including microvascular disease, pathological neuroinflammation, severe procedural hypertension, iodinated contrast load and altered cerebral blood flow dynamics. In our institutional series, 6 of 52 (11.5%) of patients undergoing therapeutic neuroendovascular procedures developed CIE during the study period. Four patients were treated for ischemic stroke and two patients for recurrent cerebral aneurysms. Mechanical stenting or thrombectomy were utilized in all cases.
Conclusion:
In this institutional case series and literature review of animal and human data, we identified numerous shared risk factors for CIE and BBBd, including microvascular disease, increased procedure length, large contrast volumes, severe intraoperative hypertension and use of mechanical devices that may induce iatrogenic endothelial injury.
Samples of Silica Springs allophane from Tongariro National Park, New Zealand, having Al/Si atomic ratios in the range 1.1-1.9, were studied by 27Al nuclear magnetic resonance (NMR) spectroscopy with high field strength (9.4 and 11.7 T) and fast magic-angle spinning (MAS) (9-13 kHz). Spectra for all samples show peaks for 6- and 4-coordinate Al and also for 5-coordinate Al. For 1 sample, the peak for 5-coordinate Al is dominant. Use of 2 instruments and 2 field strengths allowed the integrity of the spectra and the assignment of 5-coordinate Al to be verified. The “true” chemical shift (after a small correction for quadrupolar shift) observed for 5-coordinate Al in Silica Springs allophane is 36 ± 1 ppm, which is consistent with shifts reported for 5-coordination in well-characterized crystalline structures. We suggest that 5-coordination in Silica Springs allophane is associated with the edges of fragments of incomplete octahedral sheets that are bonded to disordered, though more complete, curved tetrahedral sheets in the primary particles of this allophane. Other allophanes with Al/Si < 2, and which are poor in octahedra relative to tetrahedra, may also have significant Al in 5-coordinate sites.
Symptoms and cognition are both utilized as indicators of recovery following pediatric concussion, yet their interrelationship is not well understood. This study aimed to investigate: 1) the association of post-concussion symptom burden and cognitive outcomes (processing speed and executive functioning [EF]) at 4 and 12 weeks after pediatric concussion, and 2) the moderating effect of sex on this association.
Methods:
This prospective, multicenter cohort study included participants aged 5.00–17.99 years with acute concussion presenting to four Emergency Departments of the Pediatric Emergency Research Canada network. Five processing speed and EF tasks and the Post-Concussion Symptom Inventory (PCSI; symptom burden, defined as the difference between post-injury and retrospective [pre-injury] scores) were administered at 4 and 12 weeks post-concussion. Generalized least squares models were conducted with task performances as dependent variables and PCSI and PCSI*sex interaction as the main predictors, with important pre-injury demographic and injury characteristics as covariates.
Results:
311 children (65.0% males; median age = 11.92 [IQR = 9.14–14.21 years]) were included in the analysis. After adjusting for covariates, higher symptom burden was associated with lower Backward Digit Span (χ2 = 9.85, p = .043) and Verbal Fluency scores (χ2 = 10.48, p = .033) across time points; these associations were not moderated by sex, ps ≥ .20. Symptom burden was not associated with performance on the Coding, Continuous Performance Test, and Color-Word Interference scores, ps ≥ .17.
Conclusions:
Higher symptom burden is associated with lower working memory and cognitive flexibility following pediatric concussion, yet these associations were not moderated by sex. Findings may inform concussion management by emphasizing the importance of multifaceted assessments of EF.
Usually it is a matter of systematic and patient collection of data, testing hypotheses that consolidate our knowledge in the vicinity of what we already know. We record the effect of altering this or that experimental condition, and gradually the area of scientific terra firma encroaches on the ocean of ignorance: slowly but surely the shoreline extends, creating the branched causeway we regard as Truth.
Structurally, the human brain is a mess. The problem is the way in which it has evolved: the bulk of what fills our skulls – the telencephalon, and especially the cerebral cortex – is relatively new, but it has not displaced the older and simpler structures seen in reptiles. Rather, the brain has evolved through accretion (Sarnat and Netsky 1974) (Figure 5.1): the older areas are still functional – indeed they are much the more important – but they have come to be supplemented and regulated by the newer areas, which provide improved overall integration and prediction through the massive bands of associational fibres that link every part of cerebral cortex to every other. In addition, these newer and ‘higher’ areas send and receive huge nerve tracts that have elbowed the older structures aside and distorted their shapes, making the relationships between their parts hard to discern.
There are fashions in science just as in everything else, and the popularity of measuring the time required to make a response to a stimulus has waxed and waned over the years. The earliest investigators were motivated in part by a practical problem in astronomy, the difficulty of estimating the exact moment at which a star or planet reached the cross-hairs of a transit telescope (Wolf 1865, Mollon and Perkins 1996). This work revealed two striking kinds of variability. On average, different observers had very different reaction times (Henmon and Wells 1914) (the ‘personal equation’); in addition, reaction times varied greatly from one occasion to another. Later, with the rise of quantitative psychophysics in the nineteenth century, reaction time came to be seen as an objective parameter of psychological performance that could be obtained without very advanced recording technology (Boring 1929). But these data proved easier to obtain than to explain, and reaction times fell out of favour.