Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T15:20:52.527Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - What can camouflage tell us about non-human visual perception? A case study of multiple cue use in cuttlefish (Sepia spp.)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Sarah Zylinski
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Daniel Osorio
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Martin Stevens
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Sami Merilaita
Affiliation:
Åbo Akademi University, Finland
Get access

Summary

Processes in the psychological plane cause us to overlook the fact that in the physical plane all optical effects whatsoever are fundamentally due to differences of colour and brightness, and of light and shade.

Cott (1940, p. 3)

Accounts of camouflage reflect basic concepts about the relationship between sensory perception and the physical world. The twist is that whereas the discussion of this question normally refers to human perception we must now focus on non-human species. Cott's (1940) book on Adaptive Coloration in Animals remains the most valuable work on camouflage. Cott was familiar with the idea that to achieve verisimilitude an artist has to paint the physical patterns of light and shade created by three-dimensional surfaces. Naïve artists overlook these optical effects in favour of ‘higher-level’ objects. Only with skill and training is it possible to recover the ‘innocence of the eye’ that is needed to render naturalistic scenes on canvas (Cott 1940; Gombrich 1960). This reasoning led Cott to explicitly reject psychological interpretations of camouflage in favour of what he saw as ‘simple’ optical effects. Cott was however interested in the psychology of attention, as with the suggestion that high-contrast internal features distract the viewer.

Type
Chapter
Information
Animal Camouflage
Mechanisms and Function
, pp. 164 - 185
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamo, S. A., Brown, W. M., King, A. J.et al. 2000. Agonistic and reproductive behaviours of the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis in a semi-natural environment. Journal of Molluscan Studies, 66, 417–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adamo, S. A., Ehgoetz, K., Sangster, C. & Whitehorne, I. 2006. Signaling to the enemy? Body pattern expression and its response to external cues during hunting in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Biological Bulletin, 210, 192–200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, J., Mäthger, L., Barbosa, A. & Hanlon, R. 2009. Cuttlefish use visual cues to control three-dimensional skin papillae for camouflage. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 195, 547–555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, J. J., Mäthger, L. M., Barbosa, A.et al. 2010. Cuttlefish dynamic camouflage: responses to substrate choice and integration of multiple visual cues. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 277, 1031–1039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, D. H. 1991. Animate vision. Artificial Intelligence, 48, 57–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbosa, A., Litman, L. & Hanlon, R. T. 2007a. Changeable cuttlefish camouflage is influenced by horizontal and vertical aspects of the visual background. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 194, 405–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbosa, A., Mäthger, L. M., Chubb, C.et al. 2007b. Disruptive coloration in cuttlefish: a visual perception mechanism that regulates ontogenetic adjustment of skin patterning. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 1139–1147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbosa, A., Mäthger, L. M., Buresch, K. C.et al. 2008. Cuttlefish camouflage: the effects of substrate contrast and size in evoking uniform, mottle or disruptive body patterns. Vision Research, 48, 1242–1253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, V., Green, P. R. & Georgeson, M. A. 1996. Visual Perception: Physiology, Psychology and Ecology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Chiao, C.-C. & Hanlon, R. T. 2001a. Cuttlefish camouflage: visual perception of size, contrast and number of white squares on artificial checkerboard substrata initiates disruptive coloration. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204, 2119–2125.Google ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C.-C. & Hanlon, R. T. 2001b. Cuttlefish cue visually on area – not shape or aspect ratio – of light objects in the substrate to produce disruptive body patterns for camouflage. Biological Bulletin, 201, 269–270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C.-C., Kelman, E. J. & Hanlon, R. T. 2005. Disruptive body patterning of cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) requires visual information regarding edges and contrast of objects in natural substrate backgrounds. Biological Bulletin, 208, 7–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C.-C., Chubb, C. & Hanlon, R. T. 2007. Interactive effects of size, contrast, intensity and configuration of background objects in evoking disruptive camouflage in cuttlefish. Vision Research, 47 2223–2235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C.-C., Chubb, C., Buresch, K. C. & Siemann, L. 2009. The scaling effects of substrate texture on camouflage patterning in cuttlefish. Vision Research, 49, 1647–1656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C.-C., Chubb, C., Buresch, K. C.et al. 2010. Mottle camouflage patterns in cuttlefish: quantitative characterization and visual background stimuli that evoke them. Journal of Experimental Biology, 213, 187–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chubb, C., Olzak, L. & Derrington, A. 2001. Second-order processes in vision: introduction. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, 18, 2175–2178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cott, H. B. 1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Crook, A. C., Baddeley, R. & Osorio, D. 2002. Identifying the structure in cuttlefish visual signals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 357, 1617–1624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. J. 1979. Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gombrich, E. H. 1960. Art and Illusion: A Study in the Psychology of Pictorial Representation. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Groeger, G., Cotton, P. A. & Williamson, R. 2005. Ontogenetic changes in the visual acuity of Sepia officinalis measured using the optomotor response. Canadian Journal of Zoolog,y 83, 274–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanlon, R. 2007. Cephalopod dynamic camouflage. Current Biology, 17, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanlon, R. T. & Messenger, J. B. 1988. Adaptive coloration in young cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L.): the morphology and development of body patterns and their relation to behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 320, 437–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanlon, R. T. & Messenger, J. B. 1996. Cephalopod Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hanlon, R. T., Forsythe, J. W. & Joneschild, D. E. 1999. Crypsis, conspicuousness, mimicry and polyphenism as antipredator defences of foraging octopuses on Indo-Pacific coral reefs, with a method of quantifying crypsis from video tapes. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 66, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanlon, R. T., Chiao, C. C., Mäthger, L. M.et al. 2009 Cephalopod dynamic camouflage: bridging the continuum between background matching and disruptive coloration. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 429–437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horridge, G. A., Zhang, S.-W. & O'Carroll, D. 1992. Insect perception of illusory contours. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 337, 59–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Julesz, B. 1971. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Julesz, B. 1981. Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions. Nature, 290, 91–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelman, E. J., Badderley, R. J., Shohet, A. J. & Osorio, D. 2007. Perception of visual texture and the expression of disruptive camouflage by the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 274, 1369–1375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelman, E. J., Osorio, D. & Baddeley, R. 2008. Review on sensory neuroethology of cuttlefish camouflage and visual object recognition. Journal of Experimental Biology, 211, 1757–1763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiltie, R. A. & Laine, A. F. 1992. Visual textures, machine vision and animal camouflage. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 7, 163–167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovesi, P. 2002. Edges are not just steps. In 5th Asian Conference on Computer Vision, Melbourne, Australia, 22–25 January, pp. 822–827.Google Scholar
Land, M. F. & Nilsson, D.-E. 2002. Animal Eyes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Landy, M. S. & Graham, N. 2004 Visual perception of texture. In The Visual Neurosciences, eds. Chalupa, L. M. & Werner, J. S.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1106–1118.Google Scholar
Langridge, K. V. 2006. Symmetrical crypsis and asymmetrical signalling in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 959–967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langridge, K., Broom, M. & Osorio, D. 2007. Selective signalling by cuttlefish to predators. Current Biology, 17, R1044–R1045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malik, J. & Perona, P. 1990. Preattentive texture discrimination with early vision mechanisms. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, 7, 923–932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Hildreth, E. 1980. Theory of edge detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 207, 187–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, N. J. & Messenger, J. B. 1996. Colour-blind camouflage. Nature, 382, 408–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, G. 2006. Foundations of Perception. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Mäthger, L. M., Barbosa, A., Miner, S. & Hanlon, R. T. 2006. Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay. Vision Research, 46, 1746–1753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäthger, L. M., Chiao, C. C., Barbosa, A.et al. 2007. Disruptive coloration elicited on controlled natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 2657–2666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäthger, L. M., Chiao, C.-C., Barbosa, A. & Hanlon, R. T. 2008. Color matching on natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 194, 577–585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGraw, P. V., Levi, D. M. & Whitaker, D. 1999. Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 479–484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messenger, J. B. 1968. Visual attack of cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Animal Behaviour, 16, 342–357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrone, M. C. & Burr, D. C. 1988. Feature detection in human vision: a phase-dependent energy model. Proceedings of the Royal Society,. Series B, 235, 221–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oppenheim, A. V. & Lim, J. S. 1981. The importance of phase in signals. Proceedings of the IEEE, 69, 529–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osorio, D. & Srinivasan, M. V. 1991. Camouflage by edge enhancement in animal coloration patterns and its implications for visual mechanisms. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 244, 81–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packard, A. 1972. Cephalopods and fish: the limits of convergence. Biological Reviews, 47, 241–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packard, A. 1974. Chromatophore fields in the skin of the octopus. Journal of Physiology, 238, 38–40.Google ScholarPubMed
Packard, A. & Sanders, G. D. 1971. Body patterns of Octopus vulgaris and maturation of the response to disturbance. Animal Behaviour 19, 780–790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Portilla, J. & Simoncelli, E. P. 2000. A parametric texture model based on joint statistics of complex wavelet coefficients. International Journal of Computer Vision, 40, 49–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scharf, F. S., Manderson, J. P. & Fabrizio, M. C. 2006. The effects of seafloor habitat complexity on survival of juvenile fishes: species-specific interactions with structural refuge. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 335, 167–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schofield, A. J. 2000. What does second-order vision see in an image? Perception, 29, 1071–1086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapley, R. & Lennie, P. 1985. Spatial frequency analysis in the visual system. Annual Review in Neuroscience, 1985, 547–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherrard, K. M. 2000. Cuttlebone morphology limits habitat depth in eleven species of Sepia (Cephalopoda: Sepiidae). Biological Bulletin, 198, 404–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, M. & Cuthill, I. C. 2006. Disruptive coloration, crypsis and edge detection in early visual processing. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 2141–2147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. & Merilaita, S. 2009a. Defining disruptive coloration and distinguishing its functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 481–488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. & Merilaita, S. 2009b. Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 423–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M., Yule, D. H. & Ruxton, G. D. 2008. Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 275, 2639–2643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoerig, P. 1998. Wavelength information processing versus color perception: evidence from blindsight and color-blind sight. In Color Vision: Perspectives from Different Disciplines, eds. Backhaus, W. G. K., Kliegl, R. & Werner, J. S.Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, pp. 131–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoner, A. W. & Titgen, R. H. 2003. Biological structures and bottom type influence habitat choices made by Alaska flatfishes. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 292, 43–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, A. M., Haddock, S. H. D. & Johnsen, S. 2007. Comparative visual acuity of coleoid cephalopods. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 47, 808–814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troje, N. F., Huber, L., Loidolt, M., Aust, U. & Fieder, M. 1999. Categorical learning in pigeons: the role of texture and shape in complex static stimuli. Vision Research, 39, 353–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troscianko, T., Benton, C. P., Lovell, P. G., Tolhurst, D. J. & Pizlo, Z. 2009. Camouflage and visual perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 449–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victor, J. D., Conte, M. M. & Chubb, C. 2005. Interaction of luminance and higher-order statistics in texture discrimination. Vision Research, 45, 311–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J. J., Pierce, G. J., Boyle, P. R.et al. 2003. Spatial and temporal patterns of cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) abundance and environmental influences: a case study using trawl fishery data in French Atlantic coastal, English Channel, and adjacent waters. ICES Journal of Marine Science, 60, 1149–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanuki, N., Kawamura, G., Kaneuchi, S. & Iwashita, T. 2000. Role of vision in behavior, visual field, and visual acuity of cuttlefish Sepia esculenta. Fisheries Science, 66, 417–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zylinski, S., Osorio, D. & Shohet, A. J. 2009a. Edge detection and texture classification by cuttlefish. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zylinski, S., Osorio, D. & Shohet, A. J. 2009b. Perception of edges and visual texture in the camouflage of the common cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 439–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zylinski, S., Osorio, D. & Shohet, A. J. 2009c. Cuttlefish camouflage: context-dependent body pattern use during motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 276, 3963–3969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×