We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
We have examined gyral folding in a total of more than 500 subjects with first episode schizophrenia, subjects at high risk who do and do not become ill, people with learning disabilities (LD) with and without schizophrenia, and LD with schizotypal or autistic features, as well as appropriate healthy controls.
Methods
The gyrification index (GI), the ratio of the inner and outer cortical surface contours, was hand-traced bilaterally on every second 1.88-mm image slice throughout the brain in about 100 scans. We then developed an Automated-GI (A-GI) approach to determine cortical folding in pre-frontal lobes, and have applied this to the other scans.
Results
Gyrification index values were significantly increased in the right temporal lobe of the schizophrenic patients. Right prefrontal lobe GI values were significantly increased in high risk individuals who subsequently developed schizophrenia (especially in BA 9 and 10). A-GI reduces the analysis time, improves repeatability, has low susceptibility to scanner noise and variability. Using A-GI we have replicated hand-traced results and also found a similar pattern of increased ‘gyrification’ in LD with schizophrenia or schizotypy but not LD alone or with autistic features.
Conclusions
Differences in fronto-temporal GI might reflect trait disconnectivity predictive of schizophrenia across a range of IQ levels. GI is however poorly understood and influenced by age, sex and volume measures. Further examination of sulco-gyral patterns is required to clarify this. A-GI could be usefully applied to MRI data sets of the brain in health and disease to address these issues.
Abnormalities of orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) sulcogyral patterns have been reported in schizophrenia, but it is not known if these predate psychosis.
Methods
Hundred and forty-six subjects at high genetic risk of schizophrenia, 34 first episode of schizophrenia patients (SZ) and 36 healthy controls were scanned and clinically assessed. Utilising the classification system proposed by Chiavaras, we categorised OFC patterns and compared their distribution between the groups, as well as between those high risk subjects who did, and did not develop schizophrenia. The relationship between OFC pattern and schizotypy was explored in high risk subjects.
Results
We refined Chiavaras’ classification system, with the identification of a previously unreported variant of OFC surface structure. There were significant differences in distribution of OFC patterns between high risk subjects who did or did not develop schizophrenia as well as between the first episode of schizophrenia group and healthy controls. Within the high risk group, possession of OFC Type III was associated with higher ratings on the Structured Inventory for Schizotypy (SIS) psychotic factor.
Conclusions
Our results suggest that OFC Type III is associated with psychotic features before the development of schizophrenia. Characterisation of OFC morphology may have a role in the identification of those at greatest risk of developing schizophrenia.
Negative symptoms are perhaps the most disabling feature of schizophrenia. Their pathogenesis remains poorly understood and it has been difficult to assess their development over time with imaging techniques.
Aims
To examine, using tensor-based structural imaging techniques, whether there are regions of progressive grey matter volume change associated with the development of negative symptoms.
Method
A total of 43 adolescents at risk of psychosis were examined using magnetic resonance imaging and whole brain tensor-based morphometry at two time points, 6 years apart.
Results
When comparing the individuals with significant negative symptoms with the remaining participants, we identified five regions of significant grey matter tissue loss over the 6-year period. These regions included the left temporal lobe, the left cerebellum, the left posterior cingulate and the left inferior parietal sulcus.
Conclusions
Negative symptoms are associated with longitudinal grey matter tissue loss. The regions identified include areas associated with psychotic symptoms more generally but also include regions uniquely associated with negative symptoms.
Previous behavioural and neuroimaging studies of emotion processing in autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) have focused on the use of facial stimuli. To date, however, no studies have examined emotion processing in autism across a broad range of social signals.
Method
This study addressed this issue by investigating emotion processing in a group of 23 adults with ASD and 23 age- and gender-matched controls. Recognition of basic emotions (‘happiness’, ‘sadness’, ‘anger’, disgust' and ‘fear’) was assessed from facial, body movement and vocal stimuli. The ability to make social judgements (such as approachability) from facial stimuli was also investigated.
Results
Significant deficits in emotion recognition were found in the ASD group relative to the control group across all stimulus domains (faces, body movements and voices). These deficits were seen across a range of emotions. The ASD group were also impaired in making social judgements compared to the control group and this correlated with impairments in basic emotion recognition.
Conclusions
This study demonstrates that there are significant and broad-ranging deficits in emotion processing in ASD present across a range of stimulus domains and in the auditory and visual modality; they cannot therefore be accounted for simply in terms of impairments in face processing or in the visual modality alone. These results identify a core deficit affecting the processing of a wide range of emotional information in ASD, which contributes to the impairments in social function seen in people with this condition.
Twenty-seven babies from one deprived housing area in Glasgow were followed-up regularly, for periods varying between 2 months and 11 months (mean 7 months), in a prospective study of the viruses to be found in their stools by electron microscopy. Weekly stool specimens were collected in the home together with a history of the baby's health. Additional stool specimens were obtained, up to a maximum of one per day, during admissions to hospital. Over 500 specimens were obtained at home and another 320 in hospital. A wide variety of viruses (over 200 recognizates) were detected and it has been possible to plot their temporal relation to disease episodes. It became apparent that virus excretion was frequently unaccompanied by evidence of illness and it has not been possible to describe a typical illness syndrome associated with any of the morphological types of virus observed.
The results suggest that, in one area of Glasgow at least, patterns of virus excretion in young babies are complex and will need further elucidation before the need for a vaccine to prevent infantile diarrhoea could be defined.
There is evidence to suggest that among young people with mild intellectual disability there are those whose cognitive difficulties may predict the subsequent manifestation of a schizophrenic phenotype. It is suggested that they may be detectable by simple means.
Aims
To gain adequate cooperation from educational services, parents and students so as to recruit a sufficiently large sample to test the above hypothesis, and to examine the hypothesis in the light of the findings.
Method
The sample was screened with appropriate instruments, and groups hypothesised as being likely or not likely to have the phenotype were compared in terms of psychopathology and neuropsychology.
Results
Simple screening methods detect a sample whose psychopathological and neuropsychological profile is consistent with an extended phenotype of schizophrenia.
Conclusions
Difficulties experienced by some young people with mild and borderline intellectual disability are associated with enhanced liability to schizophrenia. Clinical methods can both identify those with this extended phenotype and predict those in whom psychosis will occur.
Perceptual symbol systems form a theoretically plausible alternative to amodal symbol systems. At this point it is unclear whether there is any truly diagnostic empirical evidence to decide between these systems. We outline some possible avenues of research in the domain of language comprehension that might yield such evidence. Language comprehension will be an important arena for tests of the two types of symbol systems.
The observation that molybdenum (Mo) uptake by plants decreases with increasing concentrations of sulfate was first reported for tomato plants (Lycopersicon esculentum Mill.) in solution culture (Stout and Meagher, 1948). Stout et al. (1951) confirmed that observation with tomato plants grown in tissue culture (Table 14.1) and with tomatoes and peas (Pisum sativum L.) in soil (Tables 14.2 and 14.3). They attributed the action of sulfate ions in suppressing Mo uptake to direct competition between two divalent anions of similar sizes.
Since the report by Stout and co-workers, the effects of sulfur (S) to decrease Mo uptake have been reported in many species grown under a wide range of conditions, including vegetable crops such as beans (Phaseolus vulgaris L.) (Widdowson, 1966), Brussels sprouts (Brassica oleracea L. Gemmifera Group) (Gupta and Cutcliffe, 1968; Gupta, 1969; Gupta and Munro, 1969), cauliflower (Brassica oleracea L. Botrytis Group) (Mulder, 1954), cauliflower and lettuce (Lactuca sativa L.) (Plant, 1956), peas (Reisenauer, 1963; Gupta and Gupta, 1972), and peas and tomatoes (Stout et al. 1951). Similar relations between Mo and S have been found in forages such as berseem (Trifolium alexandrinum L.) (Pasricha and Randhawa, 1972; Shukla and Pathak, 1973; Sisodia, Sawarkar, and Rai, 1975; Pasricha et al., 1977;
Perhaps the most remarkable of the various forms of decorated ‘Samian’ vessels is the jug or vase which is the main subject of this paper. Previously, the precise shape of these vessels was a matter of doubt, the recorded fragments being incomplete, generally lacking neck, handle and base. An attempt was made in 1933 to restore fragments existing in London on the lines of the St. Rémy green-glazed form, Déchelette 62, but this restoration has been found to be only approximately correct for the particular fragments which belong to Hermet's decorated form 15, as shown in his work La Graufesenque (Condatomago).
This paper is mainly intended to be read as supplementary to ‘Roman London; Its Initial Occupation as evidenced by early types of Terra Sigillata’, by Mr. T. Davies Pryce and Dr. Felix Oswald (Archaeologia, lxxviii), and as emphasizing the conclusions reached in that paper by furnishing some additional examples of the Tiberio-Claudian and Claudian styles of decoration found in London. Mr. Davies Pryce has very kindly read the proofs, and suggestions made by him have been incorporated.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.