Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T17:12:05.979Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Moral disciplining: The cognitive and evolutionary foundations of puritanical morality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2022

Léo Fitouchi
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. leo.fitouchi@gmail.com; https://sites.google.com/view/leofitouchi jeanbaptistrandre@gmail.com; http://jb.homepage.free.fr/ nbaumard@gmail.com; https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Jean-Baptiste André
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. leo.fitouchi@gmail.com; https://sites.google.com/view/leofitouchi jeanbaptistrandre@gmail.com; http://jb.homepage.free.fr/ nbaumard@gmail.com; https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Nicolas Baumard
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. leo.fitouchi@gmail.com; https://sites.google.com/view/leofitouchi jeanbaptistrandre@gmail.com; http://jb.homepage.free.fr/ nbaumard@gmail.com; https://nicolasbaumards.org/

Abstract

Why do many societies moralize apparently harmless pleasures, such as lust, gluttony, alcohol, drugs, and even music and dance? Why do they erect temperance, asceticism, sobriety, modesty, and piety as cardinal moral virtues? According to existing theories, this puritanical morality cannot be reduced to concerns for harm and fairness: It must emerge from cognitive systems that did not evolve for cooperation (e.g., disgust-based “purity” concerns). Here, we argue that, despite appearances, puritanical morality is no exception to the cooperative function of moral cognition. It emerges in response to a key feature of cooperation, namely that cooperation is (ultimately) a long-term strategy, requiring (proximately) the self-control of appetites for immediate gratification. Puritanical moralizations condemn behaviors which, although inherently harmless, are perceived as indirectly facilitating uncooperative behaviors, by impairing the self-control required to refrain from cheating. Drinking, drugs, immodest clothing, and unruly music and dance are condemned as stimulating short-term impulses, thus facilitating uncooperative behaviors (e.g., violence, adultery, free-riding). Overindulgence in harmless bodily pleasures (e.g., masturbation, gluttony) is perceived as making people slave to their urges, thus altering abilities to resist future antisocial temptations. Daily self-discipline, ascetic temperance, and pious ritual observance are perceived as cultivating the self-control required to honor prosocial obligations. We review psychological, historical, and ethnographic evidence supporting this account. We use this theory to explain the fall of puritanism in western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) societies, and discuss the cultural evolution of puritanical norms. Explaining puritanical norms does not require adding mechanisms unrelated to cooperation in our models of the moral mind.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally.

References

Aarøe, L., & Petersen, M. B. (2014). Crowding out culture: Scandinavians and Americans agree on social welfare in the face of deservingness cues. The Journal of Politics, 76(3), 684697. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002238161400019XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrams, S., Jackson, J. C., Vonasch, A., & Gray, K. (2020). Moralization of religiosity explains worldwide trends in religious belief [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5a2dbCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abu-Lughod, L. (2016). Veiled sentiments honor and poetry in a Bedouin society. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acker, M. (2009). Breast is best…but not everywhere: Ambivalent sexism and attitudes toward private and public breastfeeding. Sex Roles, 61(7), 476490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9655-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adamson, M. W. (2004). Food in medieval times. Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Ainslie, G. (2013). Intertemporal bargaining predicts moral behavior, even in anonymous, one-shot economic games. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 7879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alesina, A., & La Ferrara, E. (2002). Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics, 85(2), 207234. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2727(01)00084-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Alós-Ferrer, C., & Garagnani, M. (2020). The cognitive foundations of cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 175, 7185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.04.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
André, J.-B., Fitouchi, L., Debove, S., & Baumard, N. (2022). An evolutionary contractualist theory of morality [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2hxguCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, C. (2010). Defining and exploring modesty in Jewish American women. Journal of Religion and Health, 50, 818834. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-010-9435-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrighetto, G., Capraro, V., Guido, A., & Szekely, A. (2020). Cooperation, response time, and social value orientation: A meta-analysis [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cbakzCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antoun, R. T. (1968). On the modesty of women in Arab Muslim villages: A study in the accommodation of traditions. American Anthropologist, 70(4), 671697. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1968.70.4.02a00010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2006). The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 8798. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, T., Wilbanks, D., Leong, D., & Hsu, K. J. (2020). Is there a measurement crisis in disgust research? [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/a8u5mCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atari, M., Haidt, J., Graham, J., Koleva, S., Stevens, S. T., & Dehghani, M. (2022). Morality beyond the WEIRD: How the nomological network of morality varies across cultures. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/q6c9rCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auger, N., Lo, E., Cantinotti, M., & O'Loughlin, J. (2010). Impulsivity and socio-economic status interact to increase the risk of gambling onset among youth. Addiction, 105(12), 21762183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03100.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aumann, R. J., & Shapley, L. S. (1994). Long-term competition – A game-theoretic analysis. In Megiddo, N. (Ed.), Essays in game theory (pp. 115). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2648-2_1Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, R. M. (2006). The evolution of cooperation (Rev. ed). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baler, R., & Volkow, N. (2007). Drug addiction: The neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 12, 559566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.molmed.2006.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 164175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton-Crosby, J., & Hirtenlehner, H. (2021). The role of morality and self-control in conditioning the criminogenic effect of provocation. A partial test of situational action theory. Deviant Behavior, 42(10), 12731294. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2020.1738645CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. (2016). The origins of fairness: How evolution explains our moral nature. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumard, N., André, J.-B., & Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 5978. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11002202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2015). The nature and dynamics of world religions: A life-history approach. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1818), 20151593. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beal, B. (2020). What are the irreducible basic elements of morality? A critique of the debate over monism and pluralism in moral psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 273290. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619867106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, A. (2019). On the economic origins of restrictions on women's sexuality.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, N. (2010). Pleasure and danger: Muslim views on sex and gender in Zanzibar. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 12(6), 619632. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691051003663713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belghiti, M. (1969). Les relations féminines et le statut de la femme dans la famille rurale dans trois villages de la Tessaout. Etudes sociologiques sur le Maroc. In Bulletin économique et social du Maroc (pp. 289361). Societé d'études économiques, sociales et statistiques.Google Scholar
Benn, J. A. (2005). Buddhism, alcohol, and tea in medieval China. In Sterckx, R. (Ed.), Of tripod and palate (pp. 213236). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkman, E. T. (2016). Self-regulation training. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 440457). Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Blake, K. R., Fourati, M., & Brooks, R. C. (2018). Who suppresses female sexuality? An examination of support for Islamic veiling in a secular Muslim democracy as a function of sex and offspring sex. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(6), 632638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human (pp. xv, 271). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. Crown/Random House.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (2012). Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boswell, R. G., & Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: A meta-analytic review. Obesity Reviews, 17(2), 159177. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouhdiba, A. (2012). Sexuality in Islam. Saqi Books.Google Scholar
Bouwmeester, S., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Aczel, B., Barbosa, F., Bègue, L., Brañas-Garza, P., … Wollbrant, C. E. (2017). Registered replication report: Rand, Greene, and Nowak (2012). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(3), 527542. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693624CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171195. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(92)90032-YCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2001). Norms and bounded rationality. In Gigerenzer, G. & Selten, R. (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox (pp. 281296). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brady, A., Baker, L. R., & Miller, R. S. (2020). Look but don't touch?: Self-regulation determines whether noticing attractive alternatives increases infidelity. Journal of Family Psychology, 34(2), 135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremer, F. J. (2009). Puritanism: A very short introduction. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brett, E. I., Leavens, E. L., Miller, M. B., Lombardi, N., & Leffingwell, T. R. (2016). Normative perceptions of alcohol-related consequences among college students. Addictive Behaviors, 58, 1620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brokaw, C. J. (2014). The ledgers of merit and demerit: Social change and moral order in late imperial China. University Press.Google Scholar
Broude, G. J. (1996). Variations in sexual attitudes, norms and practices. In Ember, C. R. (Ed.), Cross-cultural research for social sciences (pp. 123151). Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Broude, G. J., & Greene, S. J. (1976). Cross-cultural codes on twenty sexual attitudes and practices. Ethnology, 15(4), 409429. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. (2012). The rise of Western Christendom: Triumph and diversity, A.D. 200–1000. Wiley.Google Scholar
Burke, P. (1978). Popular culture in early modern Europe. Harper Torchbooks.Google Scholar
Burnette, J. L., Davisson, E. K., Finkel, E. J., Van Tongeren, D. R., Hui, C. M., & Hoyle, R. H. (2014). Self-control and forgiveness: A meta-analytic review. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(4), 443450. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613502991CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100(2), 204232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buyukcan-Tetik, A., Finkenauer, C., Siersema, M., Vander Heyden, K., & Krabbendam, L. (2015). Social relations model analyses of perceived self-control and trust in families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77(1), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buyukcan-Tetik, A., & Pronk, T. (2021). Partner self-control and intrusive behaviors: A gender-specific examination of the mediating role of trust. Current Psychology, 3, 111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02462-4Google Scholar
Bynum, C. W. (2000). Holy feast and holy fast: The religious significance of food to medieval women (paperback print, [reprinted]). University of California Press.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in nature and science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 637644. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., & Beck, D. M. (2001). Symbols as tools in the development of executive function. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 163175). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511581533.014Google Scholar
Celniker, J. B., Gregory, A., Koo, H. J., Piff, P. K., Ditto, P. H., & Shariff, A. F. (2023). The moralization of effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(1), 6079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chakroff, A., Russell, P. S., Piazza, J., & Young, L. (2017). From impure to harmful: Asymmetric expectations about immoral agents. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 201209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapais, B. (2009). Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, J. S., Ottati, V. C., & Price, E. D. (2013). The arousal model of moral condemnation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(6), 10121018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, C. A. (1932). Religions of old Korea. Fleming H. Revell. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=aa01-005Google Scholar
Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: A standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 11781198. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0551-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. B. (2003). Religion, likelihood of action, and the morality of mentality. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 13(4), 273285. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327582IJPR1304_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, A. B., & Rozin, P. (2001). Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 697710. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, T. R., Panter, A. T., Turan, N., Morse, L., & Kim, Y. (2014). Moral character in the workplace. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coontz, S. (2006). Marriage, a history: How love conquered marriage. Penguin.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., Guzmán, R. A., & Tooby, J. (2018). The evolution of moral cognition. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coveney, J. (2006). Food, morals and meaning: The pleasure and anxieties of eating. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J. T., Inbar, Y., & Maloney, V. (2014). Disgust sensitivity selectively predicts attitudes toward groups that threaten (or uphold) traditional sexual morality. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 218223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchlow, B. (1986). The powers of John Barleycorn: Beliefs about the effects of alcohol on social behavior. American Psychologist, 41(7), 751764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, D. (2022). Conceptual issues with the moral foundation of purity: The case of religion. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3e8bvCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruz, J. M., Boidi, M. F., & Queirolo, R. (2018). Saying no to weed: Public opinion towards cannabis legalisation in Uruguay. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 25(1), 6776. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687637.2016.1237475Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2004). Material virtue: Ethics and the body in early China. Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Ethics and self-cultivation practice in early China. In Lagerwey, J. & Kalinowski, M. (Eds.), Early Chinese religion, part one: Shang through Han (1250 BC–220 AD) (2 vols.) (pp. 519542). Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2020). Confucius. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (summer 2020). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2020/entries/confucius/Google Scholar
Curry, O. S. (2016). Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach. In Shackelford, T. K. & Hansen, R. D. (Eds.), The evolution of morality (pp. 2751). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-19671-8_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curry, O. S., Jones Chesters, M., & Van Lissa, C. J. (2019a). Mapping morality with a compass: Testing the theory of “morality-as-cooperation” with a new questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 78, 106124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.10.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curry, O. S., Mullins, D. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2019b). Is it good to cooperate? Testing the theory of morality-as-cooperation in 60 societies. Current Anthropology, 60(1), 4769. https://doi.org/10.1086/701478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dabhoiwala, F. (2012). The origins of sex: A history of the first sexual revolution. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Demos, K. E., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2012). Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(16), 55495552. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5958-11.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Ridder, D. T. D., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 7699. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeScioli, P. (2016). The side-taking hypothesis for moral judgment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 2327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112(2), 281299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2013). A solution to the mysteries of morality. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 477496. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P., Massenkoff, M., Shaw, A., Petersen, M. B., & Kurzban, R. (2014). Equity or equality? Moral judgments follow the money. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1797), 20142112. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickemann, M. (1981). Paternal confidence and dowry competition: A biocultural analysis of purdah. In Alexander, R. D. & Tinkle, D. W. (Eds.), Natural selection and social behavior (pp. 417438). Chiron Press.Google Scholar
Dohmen, T., Enke, B., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2018). Patience and comparative development. University of Bonn, Mimeo.Google Scholar
Doniger, W. (2014). On Hinduism. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Droubay, B., Butters, R., & Shafer, K. (2021). The pornography debate: Religiosity and support for censorship. Journal of Religion and Health, 60, 16521652. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-018-0732-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Droubay, B. A., & Butters, R. P. (2020). Pornography, religiosity, and social work. Journal of Social Work, 20(5), 557575. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468017319852599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duby, G., Pantel, P. S., Thébaud, F., & Perrot, M. (1994). A history of women in the west: Toward a cultural identity in the 20th century. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2016a). Situational strategies for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 3555. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615623247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2017). The science and practice of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 715718. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617690880CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, A. L., White, R. E., Matteucci, A. J., Shearer, A., & Gross, J. J. (2016b). A stitch in time: Strategic self-control in high school and college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(3), 329341. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000062CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duke, A. A., Smith, K. M. Z., Oberleitner, L. M. S., Westphal, A., & McKee, S. A. (2018). Alcohol, drugs, and violence: A meta-meta-analysis. Psychology of Violence, 8(2), 238249. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dungan, J., Chakroff, A., & Young, L. (2017). The relevance of moral norms in distinct relational contexts: Purity versus harm norms regulate self-directed actions. PLoS ONE, 12, e0173405. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, B. (2009). Markets: Red light states: Who buys online adult entertainment? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 23(1), 209220. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.23.1.209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelstein, O. E., Wacht, O., Grinstein-Cohen, O., Reznik, A., Pruginin, I., & Isralowitz, R. (2020). Does religiosity matter? University student attitudes and beliefs toward medical cannabis. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 51, 102407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2020.102407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisner, M. (2001). Modernization, self-control and lethal violence. The long-term dynamics of European homicide rates in theoretical perspective. British Journal of Criminology, 41(4), 618638. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/41.4.618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisner, M. (2003). Long-term historical trends in violent crime. Crime and Justice, 30, 83142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisner, M. (2014). From swords to words: Does macro-level change in self-control predict long-term variation in levels of homicide? Crime and Justice, 43(1), 65134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
el-Aswad, E.-S. (2014). Patience in Sunni Muslim worldviews. In Leeming, D. A. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology and religion (pp. 13181321). Springer US. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6086-2_9317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eljawad, M. A., Se'eda, H., Ghozy, S., El-Qushayri, A. E., Elsherif, A., Elkassar, A. H., … Islam, S. M. S. (2021). Pornography use prevalence and associated factors in Arab countries: A multinational cross-sectional study of 15,027 individuals. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 18(3), 539548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El Khoury, J., Noufi, P., Ahmad, A., Akl, E., & El Hayek, S. (2019). Attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge of substance use amongst youth in the Eastern Mediterranean region: A systematic review. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 196, 7178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.12.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, L. (1988). The victimful–victimless crime distinction, and seven universal demographic correlates of victimful criminal behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 9(3), 525548. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(88)90151-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, K., Dickins, T. E., & Strimling, P. (2021). Hygiene norms across 56 nations are predicted by self-control values and disease threat. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 2, 100013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1973). Some notes on Zande sex habits. American Anthropologist, 75(1), 171175. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1973.75.1.02a00100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, W., Ren, M., Zhang, W., Xiao, P., & Zhong, Y. (2020). Higher self-control, less deception: The effect of self-control on deception behaviors. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 16(3), 228241. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0299-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farré, J. M., Fernández-Aranda, F., Granero, R., Aragay, N., Mallorquí-Bague, N., Ferrer, V., … Jiménez-Murcia, S. (2015). Sex addiction and gambling disorder: Similarities and differences. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 5968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Leibbrandt, A. (2011). A field study on cooperativeness and impatience in the tragedy of the commons. Journal of Public Economics, 95(9–10), 11441155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.05.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., André, J.-B., & Baumard, N. (2021). The intertwined cultural evolution of ascetic spiritualities and puritanical religions as technologies of self-discipline. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 11(2), 197206. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2021.1881607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., André, J.-B., & Baumard, N. (in press). Are there really so many moral emotions? Carving morality at its functional joints. In Al-Shawaf, L. & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolution and the emotions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fitouchi, L., André, J.-B., Baumard, N., & Nettle, D. (2022). Harmless bodily pleasures are moralized because they are perceived as reducing self-control and cooperativeness. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/fzv43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., & Singh, M. (2022). Supernatural punishment beliefs as cognitively compelling tools of social control. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 252257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitouchi, L., & Singh, M. (2023). Punitive justice serves to restore reciprocal cooperation in three small-scale societies. Evolution and Human Behavior. Advance online publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franchin, L., Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2019). Many moral buttons or just one? Evidence from emotional facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 33(5), 943958. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1520078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. Norton.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V., & Loschelder, D. D. (2017). Does self-control training improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 10771099. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617697076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fromell, H., Nosenzo, D., & Owens, T. (2020). Altruism, fast and slow? Evidence from a meta-analysis and a new experiment. Experimental Economics, 23(4), 9791001. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-020-09645-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(4), 352366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaca, K. L. (2003). The making of fornication: Eros, ethics, and political reform in Greek philosophy and early Christianity. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gai, P. J., & Bhattacharjee, A. (2022). Willpower as moral ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 159(8), 19992006. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Self-regulation and sexual restraint: Dispositionally and temporarily poor self-regulatory abilities contribute to failures at restraining sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(2), 173186. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206293472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gan, G., Sterzer, P., Marxen, M., Zimmermann, U. S., & Smolka, M. N. (2015). Neural and behavioral correlates of alcohol-induced aggression under provocation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(13), 28862896. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(4), 573587. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0000337XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garden, K. (2014). The first Islamic reviver: Abū Ḥāmid al-Ghazālī and his revival of the religious sciences. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gerrits, J. H., de Ridder, D. T. D., de Wit, J. B. F., & Kuijer, R. G. (2009). Cool and independent or foolish and undisciplined? Adolescents’ prototypes of (un)healthy eaters and their association with eating behaviour. Appetite, 53(3), 407413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, W. M. (2013). In godlessness we distrust: Using social psychology to solve the puzzle of anti-atheist prejudice: In godlessness we distrust. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(6), 366377. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervais, W. M. (2014). Everything is permitted? People intuitively judge immorality as representative of atheists. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e92302. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, W. M., Xygalatas, D., McKay, R. T., van Elk, M., Buchtel, E. E., Aveyard, M., … Bulbulia, J. (2017). Global evidence of extreme intuitive moral prejudice against atheists. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(8), 0151. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geyer, A. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2005). Religion, morality, and self-control: Values, virtues, and vices. In Paloutzian, R. F. & Park, C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 412432). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ghelfi, E., Christopherson, C. D., Urry, H. L., Lenne, R. L., Legate, N., Fischer, M. A., … Sullivan, D. (2020). Reexamining the effect of gustatory disgust on moral judgment: A multilab direct replication of Eskine, Kacinik, and Prinz (2011). Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 323. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919881152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giancola, P. R., Josephs, R. A., Parrott, D. J., & Duke, A. A. (2010). Alcohol myopia revisited: Clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 265278. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610369467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glucklich, A. (2020). The joy of religion: Exploring the nature of pleasure in spiritual life. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin, P. R. (2017). The culture of sex in ancient China. University of Hawaii Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9780824864651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomillion, S., Lamarche, V. M., Murray, S. L., & Harris, B. (2014). Protected by your self-control: The influence of partners’ self-control on actors’ responses to interpersonal risk. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(8), 873882. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614538462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, C. (2017). Ethics in Indian and Tibetan Buddhism. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2017). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/ethics-indian-buddhism/Google Scholar
Goodwin, G., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2013). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 148168. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034726CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Moral character in person perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 3844. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goody, J. (1976). Production and reproduction: A comparative study of the domestic domain. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gorski, P. S. (1993). The protestant ethic revisited: Disciplinary revolution and state formation in Holland and Prussia. American Journal of Sociology, 99(2), 265316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorski, P. S. (2003). The disciplinary revolution: Calvinism and the rise of the state in early modern Europe. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55130). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4Google Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 10291046. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., DiMaggio, N., Schein, C., & Kachanoff, F. (2022). The problem of purity in moral psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868322112Google ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., & Keeney, J. E. (2015). Impure or just weird? Scenario sampling bias raises questions about the foundation of morality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(8), 859868. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615592241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K., & Schein, C. (2016). No absolutism here: Harm predicts moral judgment 30× better than disgust – Commentary on Scott, Inbar, & Rozin (2016). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 325329. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Schein, C., & Ward, A. F. (2014). The myth of harmless wrongs in moral cognition: Automatic dyadic completion from sin to suffering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 16001615. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Waytz, A., & Young, L. (2012). The moral dyad: A fundamental template unifying moral judgment. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 206215. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.686247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziani, R. (2009). Optimal states and self-defeating plans: The problem of intentionality in early Chinese self-cultivation. Philosophy East and West, 59(4), 440466. https://doi.org/10.1353/pew.0.0067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Hook, J. N., & Carlisle, R. D. (2015). Transgression as addiction: Religiosity and moral disapproval as predictors of perceived addiction to pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(1), 125136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0257-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grubbs, J. B., Grant, J. T., & Engelman, J. (2018a). Self-identification as a pornography addict: Examining the roles of pornography use, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 25(4), 269292. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2019.1565848CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Kraus, S. W., & Perry, S. L. (2019). Self-reported addiction to pornography in a nationally representative sample: The roles of use habits, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(1), 8893. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Wilt, J. A., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., & Kraus, S. W. (2018b). Moral disapproval and perceived addiction to internet pornography: A longitudinal examination. Addiction, 113(3), 496506. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerrand, R.-H. (1984). Haro sur la masturbation. In Duby, G. (Ed.), Amour et sexualité en occident (pp. 99102). Seuil.Google Scholar
Guglielmo, S., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Information-acquisition processes in moral judgments of blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 957971. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217702375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guillou, L., Grandin, A., & Chevallier, C. (2021). Temporal discounting mediates the relationship between socio-economic status and social trust. Royal Society Open Science, 8(6), 202104. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.202104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullo, M. J., Loxton, N. J., & Dawe, S. (2014). Impulsivity: Four ways five factors are not basic to addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 39(11), 15471556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M., Winking, J., Kaplan, H., von Rueden, C., & McAllister, L. (2009). A bioeconomic approach to marriage and the sexual division of labor. Human Nature, 20(2), 151183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9062-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gusfield, J. R. (1997). The culture of public problems: Drinking-driving and the symbolic order. In Brandt, A. M. & Rozin, P. (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 201229). Routledge.Google Scholar
Gusinde, M. (1931). The fireland Indians: Vol. 1. The Selk'Nam, on the life and thought of a hunting people of the Great Island of Tierra Del Fuego. Verlag der Internationalen Zeitschrift. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=sh04-001Google Scholar
Hackett, C., & McClendon, D. (2017). Christians remain world's largest religious group, but they are declining in Europe. Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 9981002. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1137651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Knopf Doubleday.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-007-0034-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Hersh, M. A. (2001). Sexual morality: The cultures and emotions of conservatives and liberals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(1), 191221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. The Innate Mind, 3, 367391.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, D. L., Cohen, A. B., Meyer, K. K., Varley, A. H., & Brewer, G. A. (2015). Costly signaling increases trust, even across religious affiliations. Psychological Science, 26(9), 13681376. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615576473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallowell, A. I. (1976). Ojibwa world view and disease. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hallpike, C. (2008). The Konso of Ethiopia: A study of the values of an east Cushitic people (Revised ed.). AuthorHouse.Google Scholar
Hamiltron, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior: I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harper, C. A., & Rhodes, D. (2021). Reanalysing the factor structure of the moral foundations questionnaire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 60(4), 13031329. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, P. (2000). An introduction to Buddhist ethics: Foundations, values and issues. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, B. A. (2017). India's many puritans: Connectivity and friction in the study of modern Hinduism. History Compass, 15(1), e12369. https://doi.org/10.1111/hic3.12369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helzer, E. G., & Pizarro, D. A. (2011). Dirty liberals! Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22(4), 517522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, N. L., & Dressler, W. W. (2019). Cultural models of substance misuse risk and moral foundations: Cognitive resources underlying stigma attribution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 19(1–2), 7896. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2017). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2020). The weirdest people in the world: How the west became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2012). The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 657669. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 207240. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-081920-042106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hershfield, H. E., Cohen, T. R., & Thompson, L. (2012). Short horizons and tempting situations: Lack of continuity to our future selves leads to unethical decision making and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117(2), 298310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herz, R., & Hinds, A. (2013). Stealing is not gross: Language distinguishes visceral disgust from moral violations. The American Journal of Psychology, 126, 275286. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.3.0275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hezser, C. (2018). Self-control in a world controlled by others: Palestinian rabbinic “asceticism” in late antiquity. Religion in the Roman Empire, 4(1), 927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S. E. (2007). “The ooze of gluttony”: Attitudes towards food, eating, and excess in the middle ages. The Seven Deadly Sins, 57–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S. E. (2011). Eating to excess: The meaning of gluttony and the fat body in the ancient world. Praeger.Google Scholar
Himmelstein, J. L. (1983). The strange career of marihuana: Politics and ideology of drug control in America (Vol. 94). Greenwood.Google Scholar
Hinsch, B. H. (1994). Women in early imperial China. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hoebel, E. (2009). The law of primitive man: A study in comparative legal dynamics. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., & Kotabe, H. (2012). A general model of preventive and interventive self-control: PI-model of self-control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(10), 707722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00461.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Meindl, P., Mooijman, M., & Graham, J. (2018). Morality and self-control: How they are intertwined and where they differ. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 286291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., & Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science, 345(6202), 13401343. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1251560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, J., Silva, A. S., & Mace, R. (2012). Lost letter measure of variation in altruistic behaviour in 20 neighbourhoods. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e43294. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, Z., & Henrich, J. (2021). The cultural evolution of epistemic practices: The case of divination. Human Nature, 32(3), 622651. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09408-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, Z., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (forthcoming). Magic and empiricism in early Chinese rainmaking. Current Anthropology.Google Scholar
Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 963976. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huckle, T., You, R. Q., & Casswell, S. (2010). Socio-economic status predicts drinking patterns but not alcohol-related consequences independently. Addiction, 105(7), 11921202. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.02931.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hugh-Jones, C. (1979). From the milk river: Spatial and temporal processes in Northwest Amazonia. Cambridge University Press. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=sq19-011Google Scholar
Hunt, A. (1998). The great masturbation panic and the discourses of moral regulation in nineteenth- and early twentieth-century Britain. Journal of the History of Sexuality, 8(4), 575615.Google Scholar
Hyman, S. E. (2007). The neurobiology of addiction: Implications for voluntary control of behavior. The American Journal of Bioethics, 7(1), 811. https://doi.org/10.1080/15265160601063969CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 9(3), 435439. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglehart, R. (2018). Cultural evolution: People's motivations are changing, and reshaping the world. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review, 65(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.2307/2657288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, M. (1990). Church courts, sex and marriage in England, 15701640. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ingram, M. (1996). Reformation of manners in early modern England. In Griffiths, P., Fox, A., & Hindle, S. (Eds.), The experience of authority in early modern England (pp. 4788). Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-24834-6_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irvine, J. (1974). Caste and communication in a Wolof village. University Microfilms. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=ms30-043Google Scholar
Irvine, W. B. (2009). A guide to the good life: The ancient art of Stoic joy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Isler, O., Yilmaz, O., & John Maule, A. (2021). Religion, parochialism and intuitive cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(4), 512521. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-01014-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M., & Ember, C. R. (2020). A global analysis of cultural tightness in non-industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 287(1930), 20201036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacquet, P. O., Pazhoohi, F., Findling, C., Mell, H., Chevallier, C., & Baumard, N. (2021). Predictive modeling of religiosity, prosociality, and moralizing in 295,000 individuals from European and non-European populations. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-00691-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. J., Wortman, J., Cheung, F., Hein, M., Lucas, R. E., Donnellan, M. B., … Narr, R. K. (2016). The effects of disgust on moral judgments: Testing moderators. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(7), 640647. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616654211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jylkkä, J., Härkönen, J., & Hyönä, J. (2021). Incidental disgust does not cause moral condemnation of neutral actions. Cognition and Emotion, 35(1), 96109. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1810639CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kan, S. (1989). Symbolic immortality: The Tlingit potlatch of the nineteenth century. Smithsonian Institution Press. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=na12-032Google Scholar
Karila, L., Wery, A., Weinstein, A., Cottencin, O., Petit, A., Reynaud, M., & Billieux, J. (2014). Sexual addiction or hypersexual disorder: Different terms for the same problem? A review of the literature. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 40124020. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990619CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kayyal, M. H., Pochedly, J., McCarthy, A., & Russell, J. A. (2015). On the limits of the relation of disgust to judgments of immorality. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00951CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keown, D. (2003). A dictionary of Buddhism. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, B. K., & Zauberman, G. (2013). Can Victoria's secret change the future? A subjective time perception account of sexual-cue effects on impatience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiyimba, A. (2012). Music and Islam in Uganda: Diverse opinions and practices. In Nannyonga-Tamusuza, S. & Solomon, T. (Eds.), Ethnomusicology in East Africa: Perspectives from Uganda and beyond (pp. 93109). Fountain.Google Scholar
Knoch, D., & Fehr, E. (2007). Resisting the power of temptations: The right prefrontal cortex and self-control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104(1), 123134. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1390.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knoch, D., Schneider, F., Schunk, D., Hohmann, M., & Fehr, E. (2009). Disrupting the prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(49), 2089520899. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0911619106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kober, H., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Kross, E. F., Weber, J., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Prefrontal–striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(33), 1481114816. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1007779107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H., & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 184194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollareth, D., Brownell, H., Duran, J. I., & Russell, J. A. (2022). Is purity a distinct and homogeneous domain in moral psychology? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(1), 211235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kollareth, D., & Russell, J. A. (2019). Disgust and the sacred: Do people react to violations of the sacred with the same emotion they react to something putrid? Emotion, 19(1), 3752. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000412CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koole, S. L., Meijer, M., & Remmers, C. (2017). Religious rituals as tools for adaptive self-regulation. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(3), 250253. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2016.1156562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koval, C. Z., VanDellen, M. R., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Ranby, K. W. (2015). The burden of responsibility: Interpersonal costs of high self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(5), 750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(11), 479487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupfer, T. R., Inbar, Y., & Tybur, J. M. (2020). Reexamining the role of intent in moral judgements of purity violations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 91, 104043. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R., Dukes, A., & Weeden, J. (2010). Sex, drugs and moral goals: Reproductive strategies and views about recreational drugs. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1699), 35013508. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0608CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landy, J., & Piazza, J. (2017). Reevaluating moral disgust: Sensitivity to many affective states predicts extremity in many evaluative judgments. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10, 194855061773611. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617736110Google Scholar
Landy, J. F., & Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Does incidental disgust amplify moral judgment? A meta-analytic review of experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(4), 518536. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615583128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langlands, R. (2006). Sexual morality in ancient Rome. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511482823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. (2013). The Dobe Ju/’hoansi (4th ed., student ed.). Wadsworth Cengage.Google Scholar
Le Goff, J. (1984). Le refus du plaisir in L'amour et la sexualité: Vol. Amour et Sexualité en Occident (pp. 5259). Points Histoire.Google Scholar
Leigh, B. C. (1987). Beliefs about the effects of alcohol on self and others. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48(5), 467475. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1987.48.467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leimar, O., & Connor, R. C. (2003). By-product benefits, reciprocity, and pseudoreciprocity in mutualism. In Hammerstein, P. (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp. 203222). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leimar, O., & Hammerstein, P. (2010). Cooperation for direct fitness benefits. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 26192626. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lettinga, N., Jacquet, P. O., André, J.-B., Baumand, N., & Chevallier, C. (2020). Environmental adversity is associated with lower investment in collective actions. PLoS ONE, 15(7), e0236715. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236715CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, D. N. (1965). Wax & gold: Tradition and innovation in Ethiopian culture. University of Chicago Press. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=mp05-011Google Scholar
Levine, H. (1993). Temperance cultures: Concern about alcohol as a problem in Nordic and English-speaking cultures. In The nature of alcohol and drug-related problems (pp. 1636). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A. (1959). Gusii sex offenses: A study in social control. American Anthropologist, 61(6), 965990. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1959.61.6.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewer, D., Meier, P., Beard, E., Boniface, S., & Kaner, E. (2016). Unravelling the alcohol harm paradox: A population-based study of social gradients across very heavy drinking thresholds. BMC Public Health, 16, 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3265-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lie-Panis, J., & André, J.-B. (2022). Cooperation as a signal of time preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289(1973), 20212266. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindman, R. E., & Lang, A. R. (1994). The alcohol-aggression stereotype: A cross-cultural comparison of beliefs. International Journal of the Addictions, 29(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.3109/10826089409047365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H., & Li, H. (2020). Self-control modulates the behavioral response of interpersonal forgiveness. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00472Google ScholarPubMed
Lowe, M. (2020). Religious revival and social order.Google Scholar
Lugo, L., Cooperman, A., Bell, J., O'Connell, E., & Stencel, S. (2013). The world's Muslims: Religion, politics and society. Politics and Society, 226.Google Scholar
Luttmer, F. (2000). Persecutors, tempters and vassals of the devil: The unregenerate in puritan practical divinity. The Journal of Ecclesiastical History, 51(1), 3768. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022046999002882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynxwiler, J., & Gay, D. (2000). Moral boundaries and deviant music: Public attitudes toward heavy metal and rap. Deviant Behavior, 21(1), 6385. https://doi.org/10.1080/016396200266388Google Scholar
MacInnis, C. C., & Hodson, G. (2015). Do American states with more religious or conservative populations search more for sexual content on Google? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(1), 137147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0361-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahmud, Y., & Swami, V. (2009). The influence of the hijab (Islamic head-cover) on perceptions of women's attractiveness and intelligence. Body Image, 7, 9093. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malinowski, B. (1929). The sexual life of savages in northwestern Melanesia. [La vie sexuelle des sauvages du Nord-Ouest de la Mélanesie (Trans. by S. Jankelevitch)]. Liveright.Google Scholar
Mankar, M., Joshi, R. S., Belsare, P. V., Jog, M. M., & Watve, M. G. (2008). Obesity as a perceived social signal. PLoS ONE, 3(9), 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, J. K. (2011). Lutherans in need of self-discipline: Japanese Shugyō and the art of sanctification. Dialog, 50(3), 271279. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6385.2011.00620.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manrique, H. M., Zeidler, H., Roberts, G., Barclay, P., Walker, M., Samu, F., … Raihani, N. (2021). The psychological foundations of reputation-based cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1838), 20200287. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, Z. J., & McCullough, M. E. (2021). Does religion make people more self-controlled? A review of research from the lab and life. Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 167170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. L. (2002). Alcohol, sex and gender in late medieval and early modern Europe. Palgrave.Google Scholar
Martin, A. L. (2009). Alcohol, violence, and disorder in traditional Europe. Truman State University Press.Google Scholar
Matthee, R. (2014). Alcohol in the Islamic Middle East: Ambivalence and ambiguity. Past & Present, 222(Suppl. 9), 100125. https://doi.org/10.1093/pastj/gtt031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mcadams, D. P., Albaugh, M., Farber, E., Daniels, J., Logan, R. L., & Olson, B. (2008). Family metaphors and moral intuitions: How conservatives and liberals narrate their lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 978990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, M. E., & Carter, E. C. (2013). Religion, self-control, and self-regulation: How and why are they related? In APA handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1): Context, theory, and research (pp. 123138). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14045-006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Pedersen, E. J., Schroder, J. M., Tabak, B. A., & Carver, C. S. (2012). Harsh childhood environmental characteristics predict exploitation and retaliation in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1750), 20122104. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, M. E., & Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009). Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 6993. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, M. K. (2002). Controlling misbehavior in England, 13701600. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McIntyre, J. C., Barlow, F. K., & Hayward, L. E. (2015). Stronger sexual desires only predict bold romantic intentions and reported infidelity when self-control is low. Australian Journal of Psychology, 67(3), 178186. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, U. (2013). The Hindu concept of self-refinement: Implicit yet meaningful. Psychology and Developing Societies, 25(1), 195222. https://doi.org/10.1177/0971333613477320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mernissi, F. (2011). Beyond the veil: Male–female dynamics in modern Muslim society. Saqi Books.Google Scholar
Merritt, A. C. (2013). The interpersonal costs of indulgence. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Michalak, L., & Trocki, K. (2006). Alcohol and Islam: An overview. Contemporary Drug Problems, 33(4), 523562. https://doi.org/10.1177/009145090603300401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, E., Sheeran, P., Baird, H., Macdonald, I., Webb, T. L., & Harris, P. R. (2016). Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 10751091. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000185CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milyavskaya, M., Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2021). Self-control in daily life: Prevalence and effectiveness of diverse self-control strategies. Journal of Personality, 89(4), 634651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, H. N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children's knowledge of self-control strategies (pp. 603619).Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidière, N., & Mercier, H. (2015). Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 26932698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooijman, M., Meindl, P., Oyserman, D., Monterosso, J., Dehghani, M., Doris, J. M., & Graham, J. (2018). Resisting temptation for the good of the group: Binding moral values and the moralization of self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(3), 585599. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, J. W. (2021). Why are world religions so concerned with sexual behavior? Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 1519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.07.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, J. W., Krems, J. A., & Cohen, A. B. (2018). Religious people are trusted because they are viewed as slow life-history strategists. Psychological Science, 29(6), 947960. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617753606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, J. W., Krems, J. A., Cohen, A. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2019). Is nothing sacred? Religion, sex, and reproductive strategies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 361365. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, J. W., Wongsomboon, V., & Sevi, B. (2021). Beliefs about men's sexual self-control predict attitudes toward women's immodest clothing and public breastfeeding [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/67vh9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukhopadhyay, A., & Yeung, C. (2010). Building character: Effects of lay theories of self-control on the selection of products for children. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 240250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1949). Social structure. MacMillan.Google Scholar
Mushtaq, S., Mendes, V., Nikolaou, V., & Luty, J. (2015). Analysis of the possible components of stigmatised attitudes towards depression and heroin dependence. Journal of Substance Use, 20(6), 399406. https://doi.org/10.3109/14659891.2014.934306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nabi, R. L. (2002). The theoretical versus the lay meaning of disgust: Implications for emotion research. Cognition & Emotion, 16(5), 695703. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nag, M. (1972). Sex, culture, and human fertility: India and the United States. Current Anthropology, 13(2), 231237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Najjar, L. Z., Young, C. M., Leasure, L., Henderson, C. E., & Neighbors, C. (2016). Religious perceptions of alcohol consumption and drinking behaviours among religious and non-religious groups. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 19(9), 10281041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nemoto, T., Iwamoto, M., Morris, A., Yokota, F., & Wada, K. (2007). Substance use and sexual behaviors among Japanese tourists, students, and temporary workers in Honolulu, Hawaii. AIDS Education and Prevention, 19(1), 6881. https://doi.org/10.1521/aeap.2007.19.1.68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D. (2015). Tyneside neighbourhoods: Deprivation, social life and social behaviour in one British city. Open Book.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D., & Saxe, R. (2020). Preferences for redistribution are sensitive to perceived luck, social homogeneity, war and scarcity. Cognition, 198, 104234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., & Saxe, R. (2021). “If men were angels, no government would be necessary”: The intuitive theory of social motivation and preference for authoritarian leaders. Collabra: Psychology, 7(1), 28105. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.28105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nockur, L., & Pfattheicher, S. (2021). The beautiful complexity of human prosociality: On the interplay of honesty-humility, intuition, and a reward system. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(6), 877886. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620961262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norena, C. F. (2007). Hadrian's chastity. Phoenix, 61(4), 296317.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A., Gervais, W., Willard, A., McNamara, R., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14001356CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437(7063), 12911298. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nylan, M. (2001). On the politics of pleasure. Asia Major, 14(1), 73124.Google Scholar
Oakes, M. E., & Slotterback, C. S. (2004). Prejudgments of those who eat a “healthy” versus and “unhealthy” food for breakfast. Current Psychology, 23(4), 267278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oestreich, G., Oestreich, B., & Koenigsberger, H. G. (1982). Neostoicism and the early modern state. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortiz-Ospina, E., & Roser, M. (2016). Trust. Our world in data. https://ourworldindata.org/trustGoogle Scholar
Osadchiy, V., Mayer, E. A., Bhatt, R., Labus, J. S., Gao, L., Kilpatrick, L. A., … Gupta, A. (2019). History of early life adversity is associated with increased food addiction and sex-specific alterations in reward network connectivity in obesity. Obesity Science & Practice, 5(5), 416436. https://doi.org/10.1002/osp4.362CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osiurak, F., & Reynaud, E. (2019). The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, 157. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19003236Google ScholarPubMed
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otterbeck, J., & Ackfeldt, A. (2012). Music and Islam. Contemporary Islam, 6(3), 227233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11562-012-0220-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paglia, A., & Room, R. (1998). Alcohol and aggression: General population views about causation and responsibility. Journal of Substance Abuse, 10(2), 199216. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0899-3289(99)80134-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432(7016), 499502. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02978CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parrott, D. J., & Eckhardt, C. I. (2018). Effects of alcohol on human aggression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partridge, C. H., & Moberg, M. (Eds.). (2017). The Bloomsbury handbook of religion and popular music. Bloomsbury Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F. (2016). On the practice of cultural clothing practices that conceal the eyes: An evolutionary perspective. Evolution, Mind and Behaviour, 14(1), 5564. https://doi.org/10.1556/2050.2016.0002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., & Burriss, R. P. (2016). Hijab and “hitchhiking”: A field study. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 2(1), 3237. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-015-0033-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., & Hosseinchari, M. (2014). Effects of religious veiling on Muslim men's attractiveness ratings of Muslim women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(6), 10831086. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0259-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pazhoohi, F., Lang, M., Xygalatas, D., & Grammer, K. (2017a). Religious veiling as a mate-guarding strategy: Effects of environmental pressures on cultural practices. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 3(2), 118124. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-016-0079-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., Macedo, A. F., & Arantes, J. (2017b). The effect of religious clothing on gaze behavior: An eye-tracking experiment. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 39(3), 176182. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2017.1307748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peake, P. K., Hebl, M., & Mischel, W. (2002). Strategic attention deployment for delay of gratification in working and waiting situations. Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 313326. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peetz, J., & Kammrath, L. (2011). Only because I love you: Why people make and why they break promises in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 887904. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peetz, J., & Kammrath, L. (2013). Folk understandings of self regulation in relationships: Recognizing the importance of self-regulatory ability for others, but not the self. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 712718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepper, G. V., & Nettle, D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, E314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1600234XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pescosolido, B. A., Monahan, J., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., & Kikuzawa, S. (1999). The public's view of the competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems. American Journal of Public Health, 89(9), 13391345. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.89.9.1339CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, M. B. (2018). Reproductive interests and dimensions of political ideology. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 203211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, M. B., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Birth weight and social trust in adulthood: Evidence for early calibration of social cognition. Psychological Science, 26(11), 16811692. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615595622CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piazza, J., Landy, J. F., Chakroff, A., Young, L., & Wasserman, E. (2018). What disgust does and does not do for moral cognition. In Strohminger, N. & Kumar, V. (Eds.), The moral psychology of disgust (pp. 5381). Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D., Inbar, Y., & Helion, C. (2011). On disgust and moral judgment. Emotion Review, 3(3), 267268. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Platek, S., & Singh, D. (2010). Optimal waist-to-hip ratios in women activate neural reward centers in men. PLoS ONE, 5, e9042. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poushter, J. (2014). What's morally acceptable? It depends on where in the world you live. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/04/15/whats-morally-acceptable-it-depends-on-where-in-the-world-you-live/Google Scholar
Powell, A. (2004). Only in paradise: Alcohol and Islam. In Robertson, C. K. (Ed.), Religion and alcohol: Sobering thoughts (pp. 95110). Lang.Google Scholar
Powell, R. (2013). The theoretical concept of the “civilising offensive” (beschavingsoffensief): Notes on its origins and uses. Human Figurations, 2(2). http://hdl.handle.net/2027/spo.11217607.0002.203Google Scholar
Pronk, T. M., Karremans, J. C., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2011). How can you resist? Executive control helps romantically involved individuals to stay faithful. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 827837. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2010). Obesity stigma: Important considerations for public health. American Journal of Public Health, 100(6), 10191028. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.159491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., Pisor, A. C., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Cohen, E., Henrich, J., … Xygalatas, D. (2018). The cognitive and cultural foundations of moral behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(5), 490501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quintelier, K. J. P., Ishii, K., Weeden, J., Kurzban, R., & Braeckman, J. (2013). Individual differences in reproductive strategy are related to views about recreational drug use in Belgium, The Netherlands, and Japan. Human Nature, 24(2), 196217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9165-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G. (2016). Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science, 27(9), 11921206. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616654455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G. (2017). Social dilemma cooperation (unlike dictator game giving) is intuitive for men as well as women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 164168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.06.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489(7416), 427430. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 413425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehm, J., Shield, K. D., Joharchi, N., & Shuper, P. A. (2012). Alcohol consumption and the intention to engage in unprotected sex: Systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies. Addiction, 107(1), 5159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehman, F. N. (2019). Self-control in Islam and its psychological aspect. Journal of Religious Studies, 2(1), 1636.Google Scholar
Reminick, R. A. (1975). The structure and functions of religious belief among the Amhara of Ethiopia. In Proceedings of the first United States conference on Ethiopian studies, Michigan State University, 2–5 May, 1973 (issue 3, pp. 25–42). African Studies Center, Michigan State University. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=mp05-022Google Scholar
Restubog, S. L. D., Garcia, P. R. J. M., Wang, L., & Cheng, D. (2010). It's all about control: The role of self-control in buffering the effects of negative reciprocity beliefs and trait anger on workplace deviance. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(5), 655660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., … Zefferman, M. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E30. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1400106XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Righetti, F., & Finkenauer, C. (2011). If you are able to control yourself, I will trust you: The role of perceived self-control in interpersonal trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 874886. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021827CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringel, M. M., & Ditto, P. H. (2019). The moralization of obesity. Social Science & Medicine, 237, 112399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritter, R. S., Preston, J. L., Salomon, E., & Relihan-Johnson, D. (2016). Imagine no religion: Heretical disgust, anger and the symbolic purity of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 30(4), 778796. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1030334CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, G. (2020). Honest signaling of cooperative intentions. Behavioral Ecology, 31(4), 922932. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/araa035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Room, R. (1984). Alcohol and ethnography: A case of problem deflation? Current Anthropology, 25(2), 169191. https://doi.org/10.1086/203107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Room, R. (1996). Alcohol consumption and social harm – Conceptual issues and historical perspectives. Contemporary Drug Problems, 23(3), 373388. https://doi.org/10.1177/009145099602300304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousselle, A. (2013). Porneia: On desire and the body in antiquity. Wipf and Stock.Google Scholar
Royzman, E., Atanasov, P., Landy, J. F., Parks, A., & Gepty, A. (2014). CAD or MAD? Anger (not disgust) as the predominant response to pathogen-free violations of the divinity code. Emotion, 14(5), 892907. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036829CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royzman, E., & Kurzban, R. (2011). Minding the metaphor: The elusive character of moral disgust. Emotion Review, 3(3), 269271. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royzman, E. B., & Sabini, J. (2001). Something it takes to be an emotion: The interesting case of disgust. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 31(1), 2959. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5914.00145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Haidt, J. (2013). The domains of disgust and their origins: Contrasting biological and cultural evolutionary accounts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 367368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 757776). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruddock, H. K., & Hardman, C. A. (2017). Food addiction beliefs amongst the lay public: What are the consequences for eating behaviour? Current Addiction Reports, 4(2), 110115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0136-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, C. C., Ugazio, G., & Fehr, E. (2013). Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. Science, 342(6157), 482484. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1241399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saroglou, V., & Craninx, M. (2021). Religious moral righteousness over care: A review and a meta-analysis. Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 7985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2015). The unifying moral dyad: Liberals and conservatives share the same harm-based moral template. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 11471163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2018). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 3270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868317698288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schein, C., Ritter, R. S., & Gray, K. (2016). Harm mediates the disgust–immorality link. Emotion, 16(6), 862876. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schielke, S. (2009). Being good in Ramadan: Ambivalence, fragmentation, and the moral self in the lives of young Egyptians. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 15, S24S40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilbach, F. (2019). Alcohol and self-control: A field experiment in India. American Economic Review, 109(4), 12901322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlegel, A. (1991). Status, property, and the value on virginity. American Ethnologist, 18(4), 719734. https://doi.org/10.1525/ae.1991.18.4.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, D. P. (2004). The Big Five related to risky sexual behaviour across 10 world regions: Differential personality associations of sexual promiscuity and relationship infidelity. European Journal of Personality, 18(4), 301319. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebastián-Enesco, C., & Warneken, F. (2015). The shadow of the future: 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, adjust their sharing in anticipation of reciprocation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129, 4054. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidman, S. (1990). The power of desire and the danger of pleasure: Victorian sexuality reconsidered. Journal of Social History, 24(1), 4767. https://doi.org/10.1353/jsh/24.1.47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2020). The effects of low socioeconomic status on decision-making processes. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 183188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheen, M., Yekani, H. A. K., & Jordan, T. R. (2018). Investigating the effect of wearing the hijab: Perception of facial attractiveness by Emirati Muslim women living in their native Muslim country. PLoS ONE, 13(10), e0199537. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherkat, D. E., & Ellison, C. G. (1997). The cognitive structure of a moral crusade: Conservative Protestantism and opposition to pornography. Social Forces, 75(3), 957980. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/75.3.957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shively, M. (2001). Male self-control and sexual aggression. Deviant Behavior, 22(4), 295321. https://doi.org/10.1080/016396201750267843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shternberg, L. I. (1933). The Gilyak, Orochi, Goldi, Negidal, Ainu: Articles and materials. Human Relations Area Files.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In Kagan, J. & Lamb, S. (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 183). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “Big Three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “Big Three” explanations of suffering. In Morality and health (pp. 119169). Taylor & Francis/Routledge.Google Scholar
Siev, J., & Cohen, A. B. (2007). Is thought–action fusion related to religiosity? Differences between Christians and Jews. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(4), 829837. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, A. S., & Mace, R. (2014). Cooperation and conflict: Field experiments in Northern Ireland. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1792), 20141435. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silver, E. (2020). Students’ attitudes toward college drinking: A moral intuitionist approach. Deviant Behavior, 41(8), 10331051. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2019.1596538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silver, E., & Silver, J. R. (2019). Morality and self-control: The role of binding and individualizing moral motives. Deviant Behavior, 42(3), 366385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2018). The cultural evolution of shamanism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, E66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M. (2021). Magic, explanations, and evil: The origins and design of witches and sorcerers. Current Anthropology, 62(1), 229. https://doi.org/10.1086/713111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2022). Subjective selection and the evolution of complex culture. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 31, 266280. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/4t2udCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M., & Henrich, J. (2020). Why do religious leaders observe costly prohibitions? Examining taboos on Mentawai shamans. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, e32. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M., Wrangham, R., & Glowacki, L. (2017). Self-interest and the design of rules. Human Nature, 28(4), 457480. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9298-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjåstad, H. (2019). Short-sighted greed? Focusing on the future promotes reputation-based generosity. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 199213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slingerland, E. G. (2014). Trying not to try: The art and science of spontaneity (1st ed.). Crown.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A., Fernbach, P. M., & Ewing, S. (2009). Chapter 1: Causal models: The representational infrastructure for moral judgment. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 50, pp. 126). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)00401-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommer, M. H. (2000). Sex, law, and society in late imperial China. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Soutschek, A., Sauter, M., & Schubert, T. (2015). The importance of the lateral prefrontal cortex for strategic decision making in the prisoner's dilemma. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 854860. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0372-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spicer, K., & Platek, S. (2010). Curvaceous female bodies activate neural reward centers in men. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 3, 282283. https://doi.org/10.4161/cib.3.3.11560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiegel, J. S. (2020). Cultivating self-control: Foundations and methods in the Christian theological tradition. Journal of Spiritual Formation and Soul Care, 13(2), 193210. https://doi.org/10.1177/1939790920918881CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, P. K. (2018). The difference between ice cream and Nazis: Moral externalization and the evolution of human cooperation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e95. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001911CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steim, R. I., & Nemeroff, C. J. (1995). Moral overtones of food: Judgments of others based on what they eat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(5), 480490. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295215006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, W. N. (1972). A cross-cultural study of modesty. Behavior Science Notes, 7(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939717200700101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterckx, R. (Ed.). (2005). Of tripod and palate. Palgrave Macmillan US. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781403979278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J. R., Cushman, F. A., & Hauser, M. D. (2005). Evolving the psychological mechanisms for cooperation. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 36(1), 499518. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.113004.083814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Story, G. W., Vlaev, I., Seymour, B., Darzi, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Does temporal discounting explain unhealthy behavior? A systematic review and reinforcement learning perspective. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 120. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strang, S., Gross, J., Schuhmann, T., Riedl, A., Weber, B., & Sack, A. T. (2015). Be nice if you have to – The neurobiological roots of strategic fairness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 790796. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strassmann, B. I. (1992). The function of menstrual taboos among the Dogon: Defense against cuckoldry? Human Nature, 3(2), 89131. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassmann, B. I., Kurapati, N. T., Hug, B. F., Burke, E. E., Gillespie, B. W., Karafet, T. M., & Hammer, M. F. (2012). Religion as a means to assure paternity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(25), 97819785. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110442109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stunkard, A., LaFleur, W., & Wadden, T. (1998). Stigmatization of obesity in medieval times: Asia and Europe. International Journal of Obesity, 22(12), 11411144. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0800753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stylianou, S. (2004). The role of religiosity in the opposition to drug use. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 48(4), 429448. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X03261253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stylianou, S. (2010). Victimless deviance: Toward a classification of opposition justifications. Western Criminology Review, 11(2), 4356.Google Scholar
Suiming, P. (1998). The move toward spiritual asceticism in Chinese sexual culture. Chinese Sociology & Anthropology, 31(1), 1424. https://doi.org/10.2753/CSA0009-4625310114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, E. (2012). Aquinas on the seven deadly sins: Tradition and innovation. In Newhauser, R. & Ridyard, S. (Eds.), Sin in medieval and early modern culture: The tradition of the seven deadly sins (pp. 85106). Boydell & Brewer. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781782047414.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Symons, D.. (1995). Beauty is in the adaptations of the beholder: The evolutionary psychology of human female sexual attractiveness. In Ambramson, P. R. & Pinkerton, S. D. (Eds.), Sexual nature, sexual culture (pp. 80118). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sznycer, D., Lopez Seal, M. F., Sell, A., Lim, J., Porat, R., Shalvi, S., … Tooby, J. (2017). Support for redistribution is shaped by compassion, envy, and self-interest, but not a taste for fairness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(31), 84208425. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1703801114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamir, C., Connaughton, A., & Salazar, A. M. (2020). People's thoughts on whether belief in God is necessary to be moral vary by economic development, education and age. 39.Google Scholar
Tamney, J. B. (1980). Fasting and modernization. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 19(2), 129. https://doi.org/10.2307/1386247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamney, J. B. (1986). Fasting and dieting: A research note. Review of Religious Research, 27(3), 255262. https://doi.org/10.2307/3511420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tariq, T. (2014). Let modesty be her raiment: The classical context of ancient-Christian veiling. Implicit Religion, 16(4), 493506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tentler, T. N. (2015). Sin and confession on the eve of the reformation. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 3090. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomsen, K. R., Callesen, M. B., Hesse, M., Kvamme, T. L., Pedersen, M. M., Pedersen, M. U., & Voon, V. (2018). Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 317330. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tian, A. D., Schroeder, J., Häubl, G., Risen, J. L., Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2018). Enacting rituals to improve self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(6), 851876. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tierney, W., Hardy, J., Ebersole, C. R., Viganola, D., Clemente, E. G., Gordon, M., … Uhlmann, E. L. (2021). A creative destruction approach to replication: Implicit work and sex morality across cultures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 93, 104060. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiwald, J. (2020). Song-Ming Confucianism. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (summer 2020). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2020/entries/song-ming-confucianism/Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2020). The moral psychology of obligation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, E56. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19001742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in mind: The coalitional roots of war and morality. In Hogh-Olesen, H., Boesch, C., & Cosmides, L. (Eds.), Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 91234). Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 6584. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., Tannenbaum, D., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Implicit puritanism in American moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 312320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gelder, J.-L., Hershfield, H. E., & Nordgren, L. F. (2013). Vividness of the future self predicts delinquency. Psychological Science, 24(6), 974980. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612465197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, F., Koenig, B. L., Graham, J., & Park, J. H. (2014). Moral concerns across the United States: Associations with life-history variables, pathogen prevalence, urbanization, cognitive ability, and social class. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(6), 464471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vatuk, V. P., & Vatuk, S. J. (1967). Chatorpan: A culturally defined form of addiction in North India. International Journal of the Addictions, 2(1), 103113. https://doi.org/10.3109/10826086709074414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vazsonyi, A. T., Mikuška, J., & Kelley, E. L. (2017). It's time: A meta-analysis on the self-control–deviance link. Journal of Criminal Justice, 48, 4863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vengeliene, V., Bilbao, A., Molander, A., & Spanagel, R. (2008). Neuropharmacology of alcohol addiction. British Journal of Pharmacology, 154(2), 299315. https://doi.org/10.1038/bjp.2008.30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veyne, P. (1978). La famille et l'amour sous le Haut-Empire Romain. Annales. Histoire, Sciences Sociales, 33(1), 3563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., & Baler, R. D. (2011). Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, N. D., Wise, R. A., & Baler, R. (2017). The dopamine motive system: Implications for drug and food addiction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(12), 741752. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.130CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonasch, A. J., Clark, C. J., Lau, S., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2017). Ordinary people associate addiction with loss of free will. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 5666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2017.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonasch, A. J., & Sjåstad, H. (2021). Future-orientation (as trait and state) promotes reputation-protective choice in moral dilemmas. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(3), 383391. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619899257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, A. L. (1997). Adversaries of dance: From the puritans to the present. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Walzer, M. (1963). Puritanism as a revolutionary ideology. History and Theory, 3(1), 5990. https://doi.org/10.2307/2504304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walzer, M. (1982). The revolution of the saints: A study in the origins of radical politics. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Warner, J. (1997). Shifting categories of the social harms associated with alcohol: Examples from late medieval and early modern England. American Journal of Public Health, 87(11), 17881797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weafer, J., Mitchell, S. H., & de Wit, H. (2014). Recent translational findings on impulsivity in relation to drug abuse. Current Addiction Reports, 1(4), 289300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-014-0035-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, M. (1968). The religion of China: Confucianism and Taoism (New ed.). Free Press.Google Scholar
Weeden, J., Cohen, A. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2008). Religious attendance as reproductive support. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 327334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weeden, J., & Kurzban, R. (2013). What predicts religiosity? A multinational analysis of reproductive and cooperative morals. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 440445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weeden, J., & Kurzban, R. (2016). The hidden agenda of the political mind: How self-interest shapes our opinions and why we won't admit it. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, A. B. (1988). The Trobrianders of Papua New Guinea. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Weiss, A., Forstmann, M., & Burgmer, P. (2021). Moralizing mental states: The role of trait self-control and control perceptions. Cognition, 214, 104662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104662CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, S., & Yao, P. (2018). Discourses on gender and sexuality. In Benjamin, C. (Ed.), The Cambridge world history: Vol. 4, A world with states, empires and networks 1200 BCE–900 CE (pp. 154178). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Cooper, G. A., Ghoul, M. B., & Griffin, A. S. (2021). Ten recent insights for our understanding of cooperation. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 5(4), 419430. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-020-01384-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(4), 231262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehead, A. L., & Perry, S. L. (2018). Unbuckling the Bible belt: A state-level analysis of religious factors and Google searches for porn. The Journal of Sex Research, 55(3), 273283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiessner, P., & Tumu, A. (1998). Historical vines: Enga networks of exchange, ritual and warfare in Papua New Guinea. Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl 4), S177S179. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0134CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, C. (2017). Ritual well-being: Toward a social signaling model of religion and mental health. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(3), 223243. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2016.1156556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Matsumoto, Y., Kiyonari, T., Takagishi, H., Li, Y., Kanai, R., & Sakagami, M. (2017). Response time in economic games reflects different types of decision conflict for prosocial and proself individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 201608877. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608877114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, L. H., Wong, L. Y., Grivel, M. M., & Hasin, D. S. (2017). Stigma and substance use disorders: An international phenomenon. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 30(5), 378388. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaple, Z. A., & Yu, R. (2020). Functional and structural brain correlates of socioeconomic status. Cerebral Cortex, 30(1), 181196. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeomans, H. (2011). What did the British temperance movement accomplish? Attitudes to alcohol, the law and moral regulation. Sociology, 45(1), 3853. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038510387189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Y. (2021). The religious ethic and mercantile spirit in early modern China. Columbia University Press. https://doi.org/10.7312/yu-20042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwirner, E., & Raihani, N. (2020). Neighbourhood wealth, not urbanicity, predicts prosociality towards strangers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1936), 20201359. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1359CrossRefGoogle Scholar