We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Accurate processing of facial displays of emotion is critical for effective communication. A robust literature has documented impairment in the ability to recognize facial affect in people with traumatic brain injury (TBI), but research is scarce about memory for facial affect. Disruptions in recognizing and remembering the emotions of others can undermine relationship quality and may result in psychosocial dysfunction. Importantly, the extant literature indicates that facial affect recognition dissociates from other cognitive abilities such that it is likely a distinct neuronal process. Thus, explicit measurement of affect recognition and memory for emotions may be critical for implementing and refining rehabilitation interventions. The present study examined the relationship between recognition and memory for emotions using a novel computerized task and explored its associations with other cognitive abilities.
Participants and Methods:
Participants were adults who were neurologically healthy (n = 31) or had a history of moderate to severe TBI (n = 26). The battery included the novel Assessment of Facial Affect Recognition and Memory (AFARM), Cambridge Face Memory Test (face memory without emotion), Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Judgment of Line Orientation, Oral Symbol Digit Modalities, Digit Span, FAS, Animal Fluency, and the Affect Intensity Measure (experienced emotion). Spearman correlations examined the relationship of AFARM performance with the test battery. Logistic regression models examined whether immediate-delay (ID-EM) and long-delay face emotion-memory (LD-EM) accounted for unique variance in group membership beyond recognition accuracy of facial affect and memory for faces.
Results:
AFARM demonstrated relationships with neuropsychological and mood variables in the expected directions across and within groups, with the strongest associations observed for memory for verbal information (rs = .51 to .58) and processing speed (rs = .48 to .57). Consistent with traditional list-learning tests, ID- and LD-EM were highly correlated (r = .85). Experienced affect intensity was inversely associated with ID-EM (r = -.29) and LD-EM (r = -.38) but not with recognition accuracy (r = -.10). Logistic regression examining ID-EM was significant, χ2(3) = 26.05, p < .001, Nagelkerke R2 = .49. ID-EM accounted for unique variance in group status (p = .006; OR = 0.65) after accounting for recognition accuracy and face memory. Similarly, the model examining LD-EM was significant χ2(3) = 27.70, p < .001, Nagelkerke R2 = .43; LD-EM was significant after accounting for other variables (p = .017; OR = 0.69).
Conclusions:
The findings are consistent with the hypothesis that memory for emotions represents a unique component of social cognition that is separate from recognition. Accuracy in identifying emotions, face recognition memory, and memory for emotions are strongly related but not wholly redundant processes. Consistent with prior literature, subjective experience of emotion had substantial effects on objective performance tasks, indicating that an individual's intense experience of their own emotions can disrupt sensitivity to the emotions of others. Future research should assess the extent to which memory for emotions relates to psychosocial outcomes such as the quality and quantity of interpersonal relationships.
The prism adaptation (PA) with rightward shifting lenses is a promising rehabilitation technique for left hemispatial neglect. The PA has also been applied in healthy individuals to investigate cognitive mechanism(s) underlining such adaptation. Importantly, studies have suggested that PA may primarily impact the functions of the dorsal or the ventral attentional stream, and we have previously reported that PA to the upward and downward shifting lenses leads to a significant aftereffect in vertical line bisection task. However, this post-adaptation effect, similarly to that seen in the horizontal plane, might have been modified by the presence of the vertical pseudoneglect healthy participants often experience prior to PA. Thus, the aim of this study was to test this hypothesis.
Participants and Methods:
30 right-handed healthy adults (age M=22,4) performed a computerized line bisection (LB) in vertical and horizontal condition. The bisections were performed twice: before and after PA procedure. Participants took experimental procedure three times, each in at least 24 h of break, each time in one of three conditions of shifting lenses; down, up, control. Both LB tasks (vertical and horizontal) consisted of 24 lines, each centered on 23" touch screen. The participants were asked to find the middle of the line. Throughout the experiment, participants were comfortably seated with their head positioned on a chinrest. Participants were fitted with prismatic goggles that deviated their visual field by 10 degrees. For the adaptation we used the Peg-the-mole procedure consisting of 120 pointing movements.
Results:
To assess the effect of the vertical PA on landmark judgments we performed a repeated measures ANOVA with direction of PA (upward/downward), the condition of LB (vertical/ horizontal) and pre- vs post adaptation as a between-subjects factor. This analysis revealed a main effect of the direction of PA (p< 0.001) and a main effect of condition (p< 0.001). Overall, however, only adaptation in up-shifting lenses led to significant aftereffects (p<0.05). Further, when we excluded participants who did not exhibited horizontal pseudoneglect in preadaptation LB, the effect of PA in downshifting PA emerged in vertical LB (p<0.05). Further, this group also exhibited the aftereffect of PA in up-shifting lenses for the horizontal (p<0.01) and the vertical LB (p<0.05). Additionally, these participants exhibited a congruent tendency after upward and downward PA, and tended to allocate their attention more upward and rightward.
Conclusions:
The results of this study confirm that the vertical PA evokes a visuo-spatial bias. Moreover, the PA aftereffect seems to be modified by the presence of the pre-adaptation pseudoneglect. Whereas the mechanism inducing this bias is not fully known, it might be explained in light of the interhemispheric activation-inhibition balance. Both the upward and downward PA may primarily lead to activation of the posterior regions of the right hemisphere, and this activation may result with the upward and rightward bias in the LB task. However, future research with neuroimaging techniques is needed to test this hypothesis.
The locus coeruleus (LC) plays a key role in cognitive processes such as attention, executive function, and memory. The LC has been identified as an early site of tau accumulation in Alzheimer’s disease (AD). LC neurons are thought to survive, albeit with limited functionality, until later stages of the disease, though how exactly this limited functionality impacts cognition through the course of AD is still poorly understood. We investigated the interactive effects of an imaging biomarker of the LC and AD-related cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers on attention, executive function, and memory.
Participants and Methods:
We recruited 67 older adults from the San Diego community (mean age=74.52 years; 38 cognitively normal, 23 with mild cognitive impairment, and 6 with probable AD). Participants had LC-sensitive magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) used to obtain a measure of LC signal relative to surrounding tissue, with lower LC signal possibly indicating limited functionality. Participants also underwent a lumbar puncture to obtain CSF measurements of amyloid-beta 42 (Ab42) and phosphorylated tau (p-tau). We calculated the p-tau/Ab42 ratio, which is positively correlated with AD progression. Finally, participants were administered a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, and cognitive composites were created for attention (Digit Symbol, Digit Span Forward, Trails A), executive function (Digit Span Backward, Trails B, Color-Word Inhibition Switching), and two measures of verbal memory [learning (CVLT List A 1-5, Logical Memory Immediate Recall) and delay (CVLT Long Free Recall, Logical Memory Delayed Recall)]. Four multiple linear regressions modeled the relationship between each composite with age, gender, education, p-tau/Ab42, average LC contrast, and interactions between average LC contrast and p-tau/Ab42. For models that were statistically significant, additional regressions were assessed to determine which segment of the LC (caudal, middle, rostral) contributed to the relationship.
Results:
Our model predicted attention (p=.001, R2=.298) with main effects of average LC signal, p-tau/Ab42, and LC by p-tau/Ab42 interaction. Follow-up regressions revealed that each LC segment contributes to this relationship. Our model predicted executive function (p=.006, R2=.262) with a main effect of average LC signal and LC by p-tau/Ab42 interaction. Follow-up regressions revealed that this relationship was limited to the caudal and middle LC. Our models predicted both verbal learning (p<.001, R2=.512) and delayed memory (p<.001, R2=.364); both with main effects of gender and education. Follow-up regressions revealed that the rostral LC signal interacts with p-tau/Ab42 to predict both verbal learning and delayed memory. For all interactions, those with low p-tau/Ab42 exhibited a positive relationship between LC signal and cognition, whereas those with higher p-tau/Ab42 showed a negative relationship.
Conclusions:
MR-assessed LC signal relates to attention, executive function, and verbal learning and memory in a manner that depends on CSF levels of p-tau and Ab42. The relationship between LC signal and cognition is positive at low levels and negative at higher levels of p-tau/Ab42. If lower LC signal indicates reduced integrity, these findings imply that MR-assessed LC signal may be a more meaningful marker of AD progression in earlier stages of the disease. Alternatively, this measure may capture a different underlying mechanism depending on tau and amyloid biomarker status.
There is a pressing need for sensitive, non-invasive indicators of cognitive impairment in those at risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD). One group at an increased risk for AD is APOEε4 carriers. One study found that cognitively normal APOEε4 carriers are less likely to produce low frequency (i.e., less common) words on semantic fluency tasks relative to non-carriers, but this finding has not yet been replicated. This study aims to replicate these findings within the Wake Forest ADRC clinical core population, and examine whether these findings extend to additional semantic fluency tasks.
Participants and Methods:
This sample includes 221 APOEε4 non-carriers (165 females, 56 males; 190 White, 28 Black/African American, 3 Asian; Mage = 69.55) and 79 APOEε4 carriers (59 females, 20 males; 58 White, 20 Black/African American, 1 Asian; Mage = 65.52) who had been adjudicated as cognitively normal at baseline. Semantic fluency data for both the animal task and vegetable task was scored for total number of items as well as mean lexical frequency (attained via the SUBTLEXus database). Demographic variables and additional cognitive variables (MMSE, MoCA, AMNART scores) were also included from the participants’ baseline visit.
Results:
APOEε4 carriers and non-carriers did not differ on years of education, AMNART scores, or gender (ps > 0.05). APOEε4 carriers were slightly younger and included more Black/African American participants (ps < 0.05). Stepwise linear regression was used to determine the variance in total fluency score and mean lexical frequency accounted for by APOEε4 status after including relevant demographic variables (age, sex, race, years of education, and AMNART score). As expected, demographic variables accounted for significant variance in total fluency score (p < 0.0001). Age accounted for significant variance in total fluency score for both the animal task (ß = -0.32, p <0.0001) and the vegetable task (ß = -0.29, p < 0.0001), but interestingly, not the lexical frequency of words produced. After accounting for demographic variables, APOEε4 status did not account for additional variance in lexical frequency for either fluency task (ps > 0.05). Interestingly, APOEε4 status was a significant predictor of total words for the vegetable semantic fluency task only (ß = 0.13, p = 0.01), resulting in a model that accounted for more variance (R2 = 0.25, F(6, 292) = 16.11, p < 0.0001) in total words than demographic variables alone (R2 = 0.23, F(5, 293) = 17.75, p < 0.0001).
Conclusions:
Unsurprisingly, we found that age, AMNART, and education were significant predictors of total word fluency. One unexpected finding was that age did not predict the lexical frequency - that is - regardless of age, participants tended to retrieve words of the same lexical frequency, which stands in contrast to the notion that retrieval efficiency of infrequent words declines with age. With regard to APOEε4, we did not replicate existing work demonstrating differences in lexical frequency and semantic fluency tasks for ε4 carriers and non-carriers; possibly due to differences in the demographic characteristics of the sample.
The present paper concerns the linear fate of transverse perturbations in a gravity-driven, thin-film flow over a soluble substrate. We propose a reduced-order model, based on a boundary-layer treatment of the solute transport and a depth-integration of the Stokes equations, using two extended lubrication methodologies found in the literature. We obtain a closed-form dispersion relation, which we compare to a previous, fully resolved analytical investigation (Bertagni and Camporeale, J. Fluid Mech., vol. 913, 2021, A34). The results allow us to distil the essential physical mechanisms behind the instability.
The limited genetic resource of chickpea (Cicer arietinum L.) is one of the most critical issue impeding its improvement and production. Collection and characterization of chickpea for agronomic traits is prerequisite to build a successful breeding programme through expanding chickpea's genepool to fulfil the needs of farmers and breeders, such as grain size and yield which supply food to a world rising population. To reach such an objective, numerous collection missions were carried out in Algeria, covering the northern agro-ecological environments, this allows us to gather 56 national and international genotypes that report a phenotypic variability and may provide a valuable diversity of useful features. Characterization experiments of 19 agro-morphological traits were set up at the Technical Institute of Field Crops over two successive seasons 2020 and 2021. All traits were statistically significant and the cluster analysis classified the genotypes into three major group, where local and improved accessions revealed an interesting outcome in terms of seed yield and resistance to Ascochyta blight. In general, the current study's findings clearly demonstrated that plant and seed morphological characteristics could be used to distinguish and identify chickpea genotypes. More often, it might participate as a source of variation in breeding programmes to meet the desirable improvement targets.
We show that the Hausdorff dimension of any slice of the graph of the Takagi function is bounded above by the Assouad dimension of the graph minus one, and that the bound is sharp. The result is deduced from a statement on more general self-affine sets, which is of independent interest. We also prove that Marstrand’s slicing theorem on the graph of the Takagi function extends to all slices if and only if the upper pointwise dimension of every projection of the length measure on the x-axis lifted to the graph is at least one.
This study aims to develop an understanding of how different communicative strategies used by case officers in asylum interviews may position applicants in various ways. The analysis focuses on a relatively standardised sequence at the start of asylum interviews, where the communicative situation and its legal framework are explained to the applicant. Case officers use guidelines to support them with this process. Using a comparative discourse analysis of excerpts from two asylum interviews, I examine the discursive means by which the applicants are positioned in the case officers’ utterances, drawing mainly on the concepts of positioning and recontextualisation. The findings show how case officers’ instructive statements could be used as a resource not only to provide information to the applicants but also to position asylum seekers in a respectful way. (Asylum interview, intertextuality, positioning, recontextualisation, speaker role)*
This article focuses on the essays that évolués wrote and Félix Eboué presented to participants at the Brazzaville Conference in 1944 and specifically analyzes how this group of elite Africans understood and participated in debates on citizenship, empire, and rights and how they articulated their arguments about the future of French colonialism to the most important decision-makers in the francophone world. For these évolué writers, the continuation of French colonialism was a necessity with no immediate end in sight. Their arguments, which ranged from expanded citizenship rights for elites to the dangers of assimilation, captured the fraught social, political, economic, and intellectual landscapes of wartime French colonial Africa. As a result, their letters tell us a great deal about both not only their beliefs and desires for the future but also the nature of reform that Félix Eboué felt comfortable sharing at the Brazzaville Conference with other colonial administrators and stakeholders in 1944.